Skip to main content

Relationship of maternal childhood maltreatment and children’s emotional-behavioral problems: parental reflection functioning and social support’s role

Abstract

Childhood maltreatment has profound and long-lasting effects, not only on the victims but also on their offspring when they become parents later in life. This study aimed to investigate the role of two key mediating factors—parental reflective functioning and perceived social support—in the relationship between maternal childhood maltreatment and children's emotional and behavioral problems. We conducted a cross-sectional study in Iran from March to June 2024. Mothers of preschool children (4–6 years old) with emotional or behavioral problems (N = 222; Mean age = 34.06 ± 4.2 years) completed measures of Childhood maltreatment Questionnaire (CTQ), Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ), Perceived Social Support (PSS), and children's emotional and behavioral outcomes, as assessed using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). The SDQ includes subscales for emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, and prosocial behavior. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to assess the conceptual model. The results revealed that childhood maltreatment had no direct association with children’s emotional and behavioral problems. However, childhood maltreatment was positively and indirectly related to children’s emotional and behavioral problems (including emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer relationship problems) and negatively and indirectly related to children’s prosocial behavior. These associations were mediated through pre-mentalizing modes of parental reflective functioning. Additionally, the analysis revealed no significant mediating role of perceived social support in this relationship. The findings highlight the significant indirect association between childhood maltreatment and various aspects of children’s emotional and behavioral problems through pre-mentalizing modes of parental reflective functioning. This underscores the critical role of enhancing parental reflective abilities to mitigate the adverse outcomes of childhood maltreatment on children’s emotional regulation and behavioral adjustment.

Highlights

• This study addresses a significant gap in understanding the nuanced relationship between maternal childhood maltreatment and children's emotional and behavioral problems, focusing on mediating factors such as parental reflective functioning and perceived social support.

• Maternal childhood maltreatment is found to have an indirect but significant association with various aspects of children's emotional and behavioral problems, including emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship difficulties, and prosocial behavior, through pre-mentalizing modes of parental reflective functioning. This indirect pathway underscores the complexity of familial influences on child development.

• The indirect association of maternal childhood maltreatment with children's emotional and behavioral problems through the sequential pathway of perceived social support and parental reflective functioning was not significant.

• The findings emphasize the critical importance of interventions aimed at enhancing parental reflective abilities. These interventions have the potential to mitigate the adverse associations of maternal childhood maltreatment with children's emotional regulation and behavioral adjustment, offering valuable insights for both clinical practices and policy initiatives.

Peer Review reports

Childhood maltreatment (CM) is a widespread global issue, encompassing all forms of abuse and neglect of children under 18 by parents or caregivers. Prevalence rates vary between regions and types of CM, ranging from 6.5% to 54.8% [1]. Parental CM has the potential to impact the development and future health of their offspring [2]. Exposure to CM may affect parents in ways that influence the overall family dynamics and the experiences of their children [2]. However, the role of mothers in the offspring's life is significant, especially in Eastern countries such as Iran, where motherhood is considered a social and cultural commitment [3]. Research has indicated that maternal childhood adversity, assessed retrospectively in adulthood, affects the behavioral and emotional problems of their offspring [4,5,6,7,8]. Harris et al. [9] showed how maternal CM impacts child behavior from the toddler years through preschool [9]. Their findings revealed that children whose mothers had experienced CM exhibited more behavioral problems during their early childhood years. Additionally, the study by Airikka et al. [4] offered deeper insights into the mechanisms of this intergenerational transmission [4]. Their study revealed that maternal lifetime mental and behavioral disorders, along with lower educational attainment, partially mediated the relationship between maternal CM and the mental health outcomes of their children. Specifically, maternal mental health disorders accounted for 23.8% of the observed effect, while lower education levels contributed 15.1%.

Children's emotional and behavioral challenges encompass a range of disorders and issues that impact their mental health and overall well-being [10]. Emotional problems may manifest as anxiety and depression, while behavioral problems may include disruptive behavior disorders, conduct disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and conduct disorders [10]. Uy et al. [11] highlighted the intergenerational impact of maternal CM on childhood psychopathology and underscored the importance of maternal factors during pregnancy and after birth [11]. Their study found that maternal CM, experienced during the mother's own childhood, indirectly led to greater internalizing problems in her children through the mother's mental health (anxiety and depressive symptoms) [11]. A cross-sectional study found that children of mothers with two or more different types of adverse childhood experiences (e.g., physical abuse, emotional abuse, physical neglect, emotional neglect, witnessing domestic violence, household substance abuse, household mental illness, incarcerated household member, parental separation or divorce, parental death, bullying, and community violence) had a significantly higher risk of behavioral problems across all dimensions, including conduct problems, learning difficulties, psychosomatic problems, impulsive-hyperactive, anxiety, and hyperactivity [12]. Another study showed that maternal CM was associated with emotional behavior problems (including abnormal emotional behavior, emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity, peer problems, and prosocial behaviors) among preschool children [13].

The theoretical concept of the intergenerational transmission of trauma (ITT), rooted in attachment- and trauma-related theories [14,15,16], suggests that mothers who have experienced trauma are at an elevated risk of transmitting the effects of their traumatic experiences to their children [17], although the transmission of trauma is not uniform across all individuals [18]. Intergenerational transmission of trauma refers to the phenomenon whereby trauma experienced by one generation, particularly maternal CM, can impact the psychological well-being and behavior of the next generation, specifically children [19]. The mechanisms underlying intergenerational trauma include behavioral modeling, attachment disruptions, and even potential genetic and epigenetic alterations. Behavioral modeling refers to the way children internalize and replicate their parents’ responses to stress and emotions, often adopting maladaptive coping strategies observed in their caregivers. Attachment disruptions occur when trauma-exposed mothers struggle with emotional attunement, leading to insecure attachment patterns that shape the child’s emotional and relational development. Furthermore, emerging research suggests that trauma may induce genetic and epigenetic modifications, altering gene expression related to stress regulation, such as cortisol, serotonin, and dopamine pathways [20]. The concept of ITT is supported by research showing an increased risk for emotional and behavioral problems in offspring of traumatized mothers [21,22,23]. However, the underlying mechanisms driving this association remain inadequately explored. Identifying mediating factors that can explain the relationship between maternal CM and children's behavioral and emotional problems is crucial for preventing and treating these issues. Two factors that we propose to mediate in the relationship between maternal CM and children's behavioral and emotional problems are Parental Reflective Functioning (PRF) and Perceived Social Support (PSS).

Iranian families, like those in many Eastern cultures, place significant emphasis on the well-being and upbringing of their children, viewing it as a central component of both familial and societal values. Parenting in these contexts is deeply rooted in cultural norms, with parents expected to take an active and nurturing role in their children's lives. This cultural framework often prioritizes responsiveness to children's emotional and behavioral needs, alongside an implicit expectation of parental self-sacrifice to ensure their children's well-being. However, these same cultural values may also obscure or minimize the long-term impact of parents' own childhood experiences, particularly CM, on their parenting practices and child development. In the Iranian context, where maintaining family harmony and fulfilling traditional parenting roles are highly esteemed, parents who have experienced CM may encounter unique challenges. Such experiences can disrupt their ability to fully engage with their children's needs, potentially resulting in emotional and behavioral difficulties in the next generation. Exploring how unresolved maltreatment relates to parenting within this cultural framework is vital for understanding the complex interplay between cultural expectations and individual psychological processes.

PRF, or Parental mentalization, pertains to a parent's ability to comprehend and interpret both their own and their children's actions within the context of conceivable mental states [24]. PRF subscales, as described by Luyten et al. (2017), encompass pre-mentalization (PM) (characterized by an inability to engage in mentalizing, often leading to attributing malevolent or developmentally insensitive attributions to a child’s behavior), certainty regarding mental states (CM) (marked by an overconfidence in understanding the child’s mental states, leading to over-mentalizing, or a lack of certainty, resulting in under-mentalizing), and interest and curiosity (IC) (reflecting an active curiosity and willingness to understand the child’s mental states, with high scores indicating over-mentalizing and low scores showing a lack of interest) [25]. The ability of parents to understand their child's behaviors and internal experiences (e.g., distress, fear) through emotionally attuned parenting practices is well-established in the literature [26], CM can significantly influence a mother's Reflective Functioning (RF) [27]. Becoming a parent can activate memories of attachment linked to one's own early experiences of childhood adversity [28]. If the parent (generation 1) encountered adversities and trauma like maltreatment or lack of care during their own childhood, this transition may lead to challenges in forming a strong connection with their child (generation 2) [29]. Experiences of relational maltreatment during childhood can result in parents exhibiting uncertain and unstable patterns of relational functioning and mentalizing [30]. Parents who have experienced CM in the past may perceive their child's cues as threatening or overwhelming, hindering their ability to offer suitable and responsive care to meet the child's needs [31]. However, it is not certain evidence that PRF is routinely disrupted in parents with CM experience which indicates the need for more research in this context [32]. In addition, studies have shown a direct relationship between PRF and children’s emotional and behavioral problems [33,34,35]. A study found that low parental PRF at a 1-year follow-up mediated the relationship between parental attachment dimensions and children's social-emotional development. Paternal attachment avoidance and anxiety were indirectly linked to children's emotional and behavioral problems, as well as adaptive outcomes like emotional competence, through increased levels of prementalizing modes. Specifically in mothers, prementalizing modes partially mediated the negative association between attachment anxiety and child competence [36]. Research has also shown that maternal CM influences offspring development through RF [37]. Choi [38] demonstrated that maternal mentalization and emotional socialization partially mediated the relationship between maternal self-reported emotional maltreatment and children's problem behaviors, as reported by mothers [38]. Specifically, the study highlighted a significant indirect effect of maternal emotional maltreatment on children's problem behaviors via hypomentalization (i.e., a reduced ability to understand or reflect on the child’s mental states [39] and nonsupportive reactions. Few studies have investigated the role of maternal RF in children's emotional and behavioral problems, particularly in the context of maternal traumatic childhood experiences [40,41,42]. Shohrati and Nemattavosi [40] explored how maternal reflective functioning mediates the relationship between maternal traumatic childhood experiences and children's emotional and behavioral problems [40]. While their study focused on mothers in secured housing, the present research examines these relationships in mothers of preschool-aged children, incorporating PSS as an additional mediator. Additionally, this study employs structural equation modeling to offer a more nuanced understanding of the indirect effects of maternal CM on various dimensions of children’s emotional and behavioral outcomes. Despite these contributions, there is a paucity of research on this topic. Further studies are needed to validate the mediating role of reflective functioning, particularly across diverse populations and developmental stages.

In the current study, PSS is examined as a mediating variable between CM and children’s emotional and behavioral problems. Social support refers to the assistance and emotional backing an individual perceives or receives from family, friends, and the community, particularly in social and psychological contexts [43]. PSS is the belief or perception that support will be available when needed [44]. Research indicates that mothers who have experienced CM often struggle to form and maintain supportive relationships, which can adversely affect their PSS [27, 45]. CM, which predominantly occurs within the home and in close relationships (typically with parents), disrupts the very bonds that should provide support during parenthood. For example, maternal CM has been linked to lower perceived closeness with aging mothers [46]. These early disruptions may deprive mothers of essential social support during parenthood. Previous research has explored the role of social support in this dynamic. For instance, Dehghan Manshadi et al. [27] identified social support as a mediator in this relationship [27], while Hatch et al. [45] examined it as a moderator [45]. Mothers with a history of CM often perceive lower levels of social support due to potential trust issues, difficulties in forming secure attachments [47], and challenges in seeking help from others [27]. This lack of PSS can negatively affect children’s psychological well-being [48].

Some studies have highlighted the mechanistic role of social support in the relationship between parental CM experiences and children’s behavioral problems. For example, maternal family social support was found to moderate the link between maternal adverse childhood experiences and children’s externalizing behaviors. Specifically, children of mothers who reported high levels of adverse childhood experiences but also had moderate to high family social support did not exhibit increased externalizing behaviors. However, this protective effect was not observed for internalizing behaviors [45]. Schiff et al. [49] emphasized that maternal rejection, maternal depression, and social support are critical factors influencing children’s behavioral problems within the context of multiple traumatic events. Maternal PSS was identified as a mediator between children’s exposure to multiple traumatic events and their behavioral problems [49]. Their study found that experiencing multiple traumatic events was associated with reduced levels of maternal PSS, which, in turn, correlated with increased behavioral problems in children. Ultimately, social support was shown to play a mediating role in the relationship between exposure to traumatic events and children’s behavioral outcomes [49].

In this study, parental reflective functioning (PRF) and perceived social support (PSS) were selected as key variables due to their distinct yet interrelated contributions to the parent–child dynamic. PRF refers to a parent’s capacity to understand their child’s emotional states and intentions, directly shaping parenting practices [24] and influencing children’s emotional and behavioral development [50]. In contrast, PSS provides an external source of support, which may mitigate the adverse effects of maternal CM by fostering a more supportive environment [27]. Integrating PRF and PSS within the same model enables a comprehensive examination of how internal cognitive-emotional processes (PRF) interact with external contextual factors (PSS) to influence parenting and child outcomes. By studying these variables together, this research aims to illuminate the pathways through which maternal CM contributes to children’s emotional and behavioral problems, while also identifying protective mechanisms that could inform intervention strategies. This dual approach, considering both reflective functioning and social support within the same framework, strengthens the study’s rationale and provides a nuanced understanding of how these mechanisms collaboratively shape parenting practices and child development.

While previous studies have explored these variables independently [27, 38, 40, 49], the novelty of this study lies in examining them concurrently within an Iranian context, where cultural norms and family dynamics may shape these mechanisms differently. This cross-cultural perspective is particularly valuable, as both PRF and PSS may exhibit cultural variations that influence their effectiveness in mitigating the associations of maternal CM. By focusing on these variables within Iranian families, this study provides unique insights into how cultural factors are linked to parenting processes and child development.

Current study

In Iranian culture, women are often viewed as emotionally attuned and child-centered, with a strong cultural emphasis on fulfilling maternal roles effectively, which is seen as crucial in facilitating the parenting process [51]. The transition to motherhood is considered a significant developmental milestone, marked by emotional stress, vulnerability, physical changes, and the adoption of new responsibilities [3]. Early childhood represents a formative period in a child's development, and understanding how maternal experiences of childhood maltreatment relate to their preschool-aged children’s emotional and behavioral well-being is essential. Investigating this relationship can help identify potential risk factors contributing to emotional and behavioral problems in young children, informing early intervention strategies.

Exploring the mediating role of PRF in the context of maternal CM and preschool children’s outcomes is vital for understanding how maternal mentalization abilities are linked to children’s emotional and behavioral development. Additionally, investigating the mediating role of PSS in this relationship is crucial for identifying protective factors that can mitigate the negative effects of maternal CM on preschool children’s emotional and behavioral issues. Furthermore, examining how maternal CM may influence PRF, which in turn affects preschool children’s emotional and behavioral outcomes, can provide valuable insights into mechanisms for targeted interventions aimed at enhancing parent–child relationships. Ultimately, understanding this sequential pathway of mediation can offer important guidance for developing interventions that address both maternal and child needs, thereby improving emotional and behavioral problems for children affected by maternal CM.

Iranian families place significant importance on children’s well-being, with parenting practices deeply influenced by cultural values and norms [52]. Within this context, understanding the relationship between maternal CM and children’s emotional and behavioral problems is crucial. This study explores how maternal CM is associated with parenting practices, focusing on the mediating roles of PRF and PSS. By examining these mechanisms, the research aims to inform interventions and support programs designed to enhance parenting skills and foster healthier parent–child relationships. Addressing the unique challenges faced by Iranian parents with a history of childhood maltreatment can contribute to improving parenting practices and promoting the well-being of preschool-aged children.

In conclusion, investigating the relationship between maternal CM and preschool children’s emotional and behavioral problems, with an emphasis on PRF and PSS, holds significant practical implications for early intervention programs and policy initiatives. Insights from this research can guide the development of targeted interventions that enhance parental mentalization skills and strengthen social support networks for at-risk families. By emphasizing the importance of these factors during early childhood, this study advocates for policies prioritizing early intervention and family support services to address the impact of maternal maltreatment. Accordingly, the current study examines the direct and indirect associations between maternal CM experiences and children’s emotional and behavioral problems, mediated by mothers’ RF and PSS within an Iranian context. Based on these objectives, the following hypotheses are proposed:

  • Hypothesis 1: PRF and PSS mediate the relationship between maternal CM and children's emotional and behavioral problems among mothers of preschool children (Fig. 1).

    Fig. 1
    figure 1

    Conceptual model of associations between maternal childhood maltreatment and children’s emotional and behavioral problems: the mediate role of parental reflective function and perceived social support

  • Hypothesis 2: PRF and PSS mediate the relationship between maternal CM and specific subdomains of children's emotional and behavioral problems, including emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship difficulties, and prosocial behavior, among mothers of preschool children.

  • Hypothesis 1: The relationship between maternal CM and children's emotional and behavioral problems is mediated through the sequential pathway of PSS influencing PRF among mothers of preschool children (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Conceptual model of associations between maternal childhood maltreatment and children’s emotional and behavioral problems through a sequential mediation pathway: first via perceived social support and subsequently through parental reflective function

Methods

Participants

Participants (N = 222) were mothers of preschool children aged 4–6 years, recruited from psychology clinics in Shiraz City, Iran, between March 12 and June 26, 2024. Inclusion criteria were: (i) while not all children in the study were formally diagnosed with a psychological disorder, all were evaluated by an experienced child psychologist and identified as having emotional or behavioral problems; (ii) children aged between 4 and 6 years; (iii) mothers with a minimum of a primary school education. Exclusion criteria were: (i) mothers diagnosed with mental disorders or severe physical illnesses, to avoid confounding effects on the study’s primary focus on child outcomes; (ii) drug or alcohol dependence; (iii) children with developmental delays, chronic physical problems (defined as illnesses or impairments that interfere with daily functioning), and/or undergoing psychiatric drug treatment, as diagnosed by an experienced psychiatrist and child psychologist. Children with these conditions were excluded to focus on emotional and behavioral issues and reduce potential confounding factors; and (iv) neurological or organic injury of children. The minimum R-squared method was used to estimate the required sample size. Using a power of 80%, a maximum of 10 arrows pointing at a latent variable, a minimal R2 = 0.1, and α = 0.05, the sample size was calculated as 189 [53]. To ensure this estimate was met, we aimed to recruit 250 mothers of preschool-aged children. Questionnaires were completed under the researcher’s supervision. If participants encountered difficulties or left items unanswered, they were guided to complete the questionnaires appropriately, ensuring no missing data in the final dataset. Participants who were unwilling to fully complete the questionnaires, even after reminders, or provided unreliable responses were excluded, along with those who did not meet the eligibility criteria. This process resulted in a final sample size of 222 mothers.

Procedure

Participants were clients of three child psychologists, each with over five years of experience. After the child psychologists confirmed the presence of emotional or behavioral problems in the children and obtained informed written consent from the mothers, the research questionnaires were administered. All scales used in this study were well-established and validated instruments, widely utilized in previous psychological research. The measures were parent-reported, with mothers providing self-reports about their children's emotional and behavioral problems. Mothers were briefed on the study’s purpose, the confidentiality of their responses, and the voluntary nature of their participation. All participants engaged in the survey voluntarily and had the option to withdraw at any point. The average time taken to complete the questionnaires was approximately 30 min. No compensation was provided for participation, but upon completion of the study, a specialist conducted a complimentary parenting workshop comprising 6 sessions as a gesture of appreciation to the mothers.

Childhood maltreatment Questionnaire (CTQ)

Bernstein et al. [54] developed the Childhood Maltreatment Questionnaire (CTQ), a 28-item self-report tool designed to assess adults' history of childhood maltreatment retrospectively [54]. The CTQ includes five subscales: emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect, and physical neglect. Responses are rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from "1-never true" to "5-always true," with higher scores indicating more severe maltreatment. The CTQ has demonstrated good validity across various clinical and general populations. In the original study by Bernstein et al. [54], Cronbach's α coefficients were 0.71 for the total score and ranged from 0.65 to 0.91 for the subscales: 0.77 for emotional abuse, 0.78 for physical abuse, 0.91 for sexual abuse, 0.85 for emotional neglect, and 0.65 for physical neglect [54]. The adapted Farsi version of the CTQ showed strong reliability (test–retest coefficient = 0.9, internal consistency coefficients ranging from 0.60 to 0.85) and satisfactory validity, including convergent validity with the General Health Questionnaire-12 (r = 0.42) [55]. In this study, the Cronbach's α coefficients were as follows: 0.76 for emotional abuse, 0.75 for physical abuse, 0.91 for sexual abuse, 0.85 for emotional neglect, and 0.75 for physical neglect.

The strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ)—patent version

The SDQ, developed by Robert Goodman in 1997, includes five scales, each with five items [56]. These scales assess emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationships, and prosocial behavior. Responses are given on a three-point Likert scale: 'not true,' 'somewhat true,' or 'certainly true.' Negative items are scored from 0 to 2, while positive items are scored inversely from 2 to 0, with higher scores indicating more problematic attributes. Subscores for each subscale range from 0 to 10. The prosocial behavior subscale is assessed independently and is not included in the total score for emotional and behavioral problems. Goodman [56] reported satisfactory internal consistency for all subscales (α = 0.72 to 0.76), except for the peer relationship scale (α = 0.58) [57]. The Persian version of the SDQ demonstrated good reliability and validity, with a Cronbach's α of 0.73 for parent total scores [58]. Significant correlations (p < 0.01) between the SDQ and Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) subscales support the SDQ's convergent validity [58].

Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ)

The PRFQ, developed by Luyten et al. in 2017, is an 18-item self-report questionnaire consisting of three subscales: Pre-mentalizing modes (PM), Certainty about mental states (CMS), and Interest and curiosity in mental states (IC) [25]. PM reflects the tendency to make malevolent or developmentally insensitive attributions about the child’s mental states, CMS indicates either excessive certainty about or a lack of acknowledgment of the opacity of the child’s mental states, and IC represents active curiosity and a willingness to understand the child’s mental states. Responses are provided on a 7-point Likert scale. Higher scores on the PRFQ Pre-mentalizing subscale indicate lower levels of parental RF [25]. However, interpreting scores on the PRFQ-CMS and PRFQ-IC subscales to predict high or low levels of RF is less straightforward. Literature suggests that high scores may indicate high RF, but both low and very high scores may indicate lower RF, with more optimal levels often being average. This ambiguity suggests that moderate scores on these subscales may be more adaptive, while very low or very high scores may reflect dysfunction [59]. Rutherford et al. [60] conducted both exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses on the three-factor structure of the PRFQ in samples of mothers and fathers, demonstrating good internal consistency: α = 0.7 for Pre-Mentalizing, α = 0.82 for Certainty about Mental States, and α = 0.74 for Interest and Curiosity in Mental States [60]. The Persian version of the PRFQ demonstrated good reliability, with Cronbach's α values of.71 for PM, 0.71 for CMS, and 0.59 for IC [27].

Perceived Social Support Scale (PSSS)

The PSSS, developed by Procidano & Heller [61], is a self-report measure that assesses individuals' perception of available social support from family and friends [61]. The PSSS includes two subscales: a 20-item Family subscale (PSS-FAM) and a 20-item Friend subscale (PSS-FRI). Responses are given as "Yes," "No," or "Do not know," with "No" indicating no perceived support (scored 0), and "Yes" indicating perceived support (scored + 1). The total score ranges from 0 to 20, with higher scores reflecting greater perceived support. The PSSS has demonstrated acceptable validity and reliability. In the study by Evans et al. [62], the coefficient alpha was 0.91 for PSS-FAM and 0.90 for PSS-FRI [62]. In prior research [63], has also affirmed the validity and reliability of the Persian version of the scale.

Data analyses

Descriptive statistics were computed using SPSS-22. The conceptual models were assessed through structural equation modeling (SEM) with Smart PLS-3 (Figs. 1 and 2). Initially, we tested for normality and conducted Pearson correlations for all variables. Mediating effects were evaluated using bootstrapping (1000 bootstrap draws) with 95% confidence intervals [64]. During assumption testing, we observed that the variables did not follow a normal distribution (see Supplemental Table 1). As a result, Partial Least Squares regression (PLS) was used to analyze the model, as it is not influenced by normality assumptions or sample size [65]. The Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) analysis did not reveal significant multicollinearity (see Supplemental Table 1). Importantly, no missing data were present in the final dataset. The questionnaires were completed under the supervision of the researcher, and if any participant failed to understand or left an item unanswered, they were guided to complete it appropriately, ensuring no missing data in the final dataset.

The first step in applying PLS-SEM is evaluating the measurement model. This includes assessing reliability—internal consistency reliability, Composite Reliability (CR), and Homogeneous Reliability Ratio (Rho_A)—and construct validity, which encompasses both convergent and discriminant validity. Convergent validity indicates the extent to which the indicators of a specific construct are correlated and measure the intended construct accurately. In PLS-SEM, convergent validity is assessed using Average Variance Extracted (AVE) [65]. Discriminant validity ensures that the constructs in the model are distinct and that the indicators of one construct have low correlations with indicators of other constructs. Methods to assess discriminant validity include the Fornell-Larcker criterion (FL), Heterotrait-Monotrait Ratio (HTMT), and cross-loading [66].

Results

Participant characteristics

The mean age of the mothers was 34.06 years (SD = 4.2), and the mean age of their children was 4.99 years (SD = 0.85). Detailed demographic characteristics and descriptive variables were summarized in Tables 1 and 2.

Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of the Study Sample (n = 222)
Table 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations of variables related to childhood maltreatment, parental reflective functioning, and children's emotional and behavioral problems

Validity of measurement models

The standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), which represents the root mean square discrepancy between the observed correlations and the model-implied correlations, was 0.09 for the first model (total emotional-behavioral problems) and 0.09 for the second model (subscales of the strengths and difficulties). Both values are below the acceptable threshold of 0.1, indicating a good fit for both models [67].

All constructs demonstrated good internal consistency, with Cronbach's alpha exceeding 0.7 [68], CR > 0.6, and Rho_A > 0.7 [69]. Furthermore, convergent validity criteria were met, as all constructs had AVE > 0.5 [69] (Supplemental Table 2). Discriminant validity was confirmed using the FL, where the square root of AVE for each construct exceeded its correlations with other constructs. Additionally, the HTMT values were below 0.9, further supporting divergent validity [70]. These findings indicate that the constructs in the model were distinct and appropriately measured. The cross-loading analysis confirmed that all indicators loaded higher on their respective constructs than on any other constructs. For detailed results, including the FL matrix, HTMT values, and individual item factor loadings, please refer to Supplemental Tables 3. and 1. SD Standard Deviation, PRFQ-CM Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire -certainty about mental states; PRFQ-IC Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire -interest and curiosity in mental states, PRFQ-PM Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire-pre-mentalizing modes, PSS Perceived Social Support.

Mediation structural model

Table 3, Figs. 3 and 4 illustrate the direct and indirect associations of the model. The findings related to the first and second hypotheses indicate that the direct associations of maternal CM with PRFQ-IC (β = −0.28, p < 0.001), PRFQ-PM (β = 0.27, p < 0.01), and PSS (β = −0.44, p < 0.001) were significant. Furthermore, the direct association of maternal CM with PRFQ-CM (β = −0.12, p = 0.06) was marginally significant. PRFQ-CM was directly associated with prosocial behavior (β = 0.18, p < 0.05). Moreover, emotional symptoms (β = 0.26, p < 0.01), conduct problems (β = 0.36, p < 0.01), hyperactivity/inattention (β = 0.33, p < 0.001), peer relationship problems (β = 0.27, p < 0.01), and prosocial behavior (β = −0.28, p < 0.001) were all directly associated with PRFQ-PM. Indirect associations of maternal CM via PRFQ-PM were significantly related to total emotional-behavioral problems (β = 0.07, p < 0.05; Fig. 3), emotional symptoms (β = 0.07, p = 0.05; Fig. 4), conduct problems (β = 0.1, p < 0.05; Fig. 4), hyperactivity/inattention (β = 0.09, p < 0.05; Fig. 4), peer relationship problems (β = 0.07, p < 0.05; Fig. 4), and prosocial behavior (β = −0.07, p < 0.05; Fig. 4). Nevertheless, no significant indirect associations were observed between maternal CM and total emotional-behavioral problems or any of the emotional or behavioral outcomes through PSS (all p > 0.05).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Standardized estimates of the structural model. Note: The numbers displayed within the blue circles represent the percentage of variance explained by the model for each dependent variable (DV)

Fig. 4
figure 4

Standardized estimates of the structural model. Note: The numbers displayed within the blue circles represent the percentage of variance explained by the model for each dependent variable (DV)

Table 3 Summary of first and second tested hypotheses and findings

Regarding the third hypothesis, the direct association between maternal CM and emotional-behavioral problems was not significant (β = 0.05, p = 0.5). CM was significantly associated with PSS (β = −0.3, p < 0.001). However, the relationship between PSS and PRF was not significant (β = 0.17, p = 0.2). PRF was significantly associated with emotional-behavioral problems (β = −0.27, p < 0.05). The indirect association of maternal CM with emotional-behavioral problems through the sequential pathway of PSS and PRF was not significant (β = 0.01, p = 0.3) (Table 4, Fig. 5).

Table 4 Summary of third tested hypothesis and findings
Fig. 5
figure 5

Standardized estimates of the structural model. Note: The numbers displayed within the blue circles represent the percentage of variance explained by the model for each dependent variable (DV)

Discussion

This study investigated how parental reflective functioning (PRF) and perceived social support (PSS) mediated the relationship between maternal childhood maltreatment (CM) and children's emotional and behavioral problems among mothers of preschool children in Iran. The results, based on Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) analysis, were intriguing, showing that maternal CM was not directly related to children's emotional and behavioral problems. Instead, the relationship was indirect, mediated through Pre-Mentalizing (PM) modes. Furthermore, the indirect association of maternal CM with emotional and behavioral problems through the sequential pathway of PSS and PRF was not significant.

The role of PRF and PSS in associations between maternal CM and children’s emotional and behavioral problems

As the experimental model of the research indicates, maternal CM did not mediate preschool children's total emotional-behavioral problems (a composite score including emotional symptoms, conduct problems, peer relationship problems, and hyperactivity/inattention) through PRF or PSS. These results aligned with prior studies, which identified mediating variables such as maternal depression/anxiety symptoms, parenting behavior, maternal mental health, child traumatic experiences, maternal parenting styles of rejection, and maternal-child attachment, emphasizing the complexity of the CM–children’s behavior relationship [7, 9, 12, 71,72,73]. Similarly, Shohrati & Nemattavosi [40] showed the importance of reflective functioning in understanding this relationship [40]. However, while their study identified direct associations between CM and children's problems, our findings highlight the crucial role of pre-mentalizing modes in the mediation process, and that social support did not have a significant indirect effect in this context. The findings of this study may be due to the composite nature of the emotional-behavioral problems, which combines diverse dimensions like emotional symptoms, conduct issues, and hyperactivity/inattention. When these dimensions are aggregated into a single score, distinct relationships with PRF or PSS become obscured, suggesting that examining each dimension separately may provide clearer pathways. Specifically, the pre-mentalization (PM) aspect of PRF, which involves misinterpreting a child’s actions as intentional, may be more directly influential in shaping certain behavioral outcomes, such as hyperactivity/inattention and peer relationship problems. The role of PM as a mediator underscores the importance of considering indirect pathways in understanding how CM impacts children's emotional and behavioral outcomes.

The role of PRF and PSS in associations between maternal CM and specific subdomains of children’s emotional and behavioral problems

Our study demonstrated that maternal CM is indirectly related to emotional symptoms through PM modes. CM often develops immature defensive mechanisms [74] aimed at shielding individuals from emotional pain and vulnerability [75]. Mothers who have experienced CM may develop heightened sensitivity to perceived threats [76], perhaps interpreting their children's behavior through a lens of harm or malicious intent. These defensive mechanisms function as protective strategies to cope with past emotional distress [77]. For example, a mother with elevated pre-mentalization may view a child's normal emotional reactions, such as crying or frustration, as deliberate acts of defiance or disrespect. As a result, mothers with pre-mentalizing may misinterpret their child's emotional needs [33], which can lead to inappropriate responses and, in turn, contribute to behavioral and emotional problems in their child.

In further support of the third finding, the results showed that maternal CM is indirectly related to children's conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention problems through PM modes. Maternal CM often results in long-lasting emotional and psychological effects that impair a mother's ability to mentalize, or understand and respond to their child's mental states [27]. These mothers may default to PM modes, characterized by a lack of reflection on their own and their child's thoughts and feelings, leading to less empathetic and more reactive parenting [78]. Children in such environments are more likely to develop conduct problems, as they may emulate the reactive and unempathetic behaviors observed in their caregivers, a phenomenon supported by Bandura's theory [79].

In further support of the forth finding, our results revealed that maternal CM is indirectly related to children's peer relationship problems through PM modes. This means that mothers with a history of CM often exhibit these PM modes, leading to difficulties in social interactions for their children. As a result, children may inherit or learn maladaptive social patterns, complicating their ability to form and maintain peer relationships. Research indicates that PM modes contribute to interpersonal relationship problems, including a strong fear of rejection [80,81,82]. When this fear of rejection is present in mothers, it can be transmitted to their children through behaviors and attitudes that reflect anxiety and hypervigilance in social interactions. Consequently, children may develop similar fears and anxieties, making them more susceptible to peer relationship problems.

In further support of the fifth finding, the results revealed that maternal CM is indirectly related to children's prosocial behaviors through PM modes. Mothers who have experienced CM may encounter difficulties with PM, which can hinder their ability to understand and respond effectively to their children's emotional needs [83]. PM modes represent specific forms of RF characterized by challenges in accurately interpreting mental states [84]. When a mother operates in PM modes, she may misinterpret or overlook her child's emotional states, potentially exacerbating the child's emotional and behavioral challenges or diminishing their prosocial behaviors. This indirect relationship underscores the intricate ways in which maltreatment can affect subsequent generations.

In further support of the sixth finding, the results showed that mothers’ Certainty about Mental States (CMs) did not mediate the relationship between maternal CM and children’s emotional-behavioral problems. Mothers with a history of CM often struggle with mentalization, leading to difficulties in interpreting children’s emotions. They may lack confidence in recognizing emotional states or attribute exaggerated interpersonal meanings, such as anger, instead of focusing on observable cues [85]. These tendencies, known as under- and over-mentalizing, hinder a balanced understanding of children’s mental states. However, certainty regarding mental states does not mediate the link between maternal CM and preschool children’s emotional-behavioral problems. This construct mainly relates to interpreting a child’s internal emotions [25], whereas children’s behavioral problems often stem from parental responses to outward actions (e.g. Raval et al., 2018) [86]. Thus, a mother’s level of certainty may not always influence child outcomes. Additionally, in some cultural contexts, such as Iran, parenting practices may prioritize behavioral guidance and discipline over understanding internal emotional states [87]. This emphasis on external behavior rather than internal mental states may further reduce the mediating role of certainty in shaping children's emotional and behavioral outcomes.

In further support of the seventh finding, the results showed that mothers’ Interest and Curiosity (IC) did not mediate the relationship between maternal CM and children’s emotional-behavioral problems. Furthermore, CM is associated with a decrease in parents' interest and curiosity about their child's mental states, as it depletes their psychological resources, limiting effective interaction [88]. This can reduce their willingness to explore their child’s thoughts and feelings. However, interest and curiosity cannot mediate the relationship between maternal CM and children's emotional-behavioral problems, as this aspect of reflective functioning reflects the parents' tendency to understand the child [25], rather than the quality of that understanding. In other words, while interest and curiosity may reflect parents' attention to their child’s mental states, this attention, when free from bias, does not directly affect emotional-behavioral problems. In contrast, PM can act as a mediator, as it directly influences how parents interpret their child’s behavior and interact with them.

In further support of the eighth finding, the results demonstrated that PSS did not significantly mediate the relationship between maternal CM and children's emotional and behavioral problems. This suggests that PSS, despite being a relevant factor in maternal CM, may not serve as a bridging mechanism in this specific association. Although the adapted PSS is reliable and culturally relevant in Iranian samples, the dynamics of maternal CM and social support may be influenced by cultural and relational factors In Iranian society, social and familial support systems primarily emphasize practical assistance and collective responsibility [27] rather than emotional support. While families may provide tangible help, such as childcare, they may not offer the emotional validation and psychological resources needed to address the long-term effects of CM. As a result, the type of support perceived by mothers may not be sufficient to influence their children’s emotional and behavioral outcomes, explaining the lack of a mediating role for PSS in this relationship.

The role of PSS in influencing PRF as a sequential mediator in the associations between maternal CM and children’s emotional and behavioral problems

The finding revealed that the relationship between maternal CM and children's emotional and behavioral problems, mediated sequentially by PSS and PRF, was not statistically significant. Childhood trauma can hinder a mother's ability to form trusting relationships and seek support, ultimately reducing her social connections and diminishing her PSS [89, 90]. However, social support may not significantly impact a mother’s reflective functioning, as reflective functioning is primarily influenced by intrapersonal processes. These processes involve an individual's ability to understand and respond to their own emotions and those of others [24], which may operate independently of external influences like social support. Thus, while social support can help mitigate the negative effects of childhood trauma, it cannot alone play a determining role in improving maternal reflective functioning. Moreover, PRF is crucial for recognizing a child's emotional needs and regulating emotional responses accordingly [83]. When a mother is unable to understand and appropriately respond to her child's needs, it can lead to emotional and behavioral problems in the child [33,34,35]. This underscores that, although childhood trauma may reduce social support, the lack of a significant impact of social support on reflective functioning does not result in a reduction of emotional and behavioral problems in preschool children.

Limitations

The study has several limitations. First, its cross-sectional design restricts the ability to establish causal relationships among maternal childhood maltreatment (CM), parental reflective functioning (PRF), perceived social support (PSS), and children's emotional and behavioral outcomes. Additionally, the limitations inherent in cross-sectional mediation should be acknowledged, as mediation analyses in such designs do not confirm temporal precedence and should be interpreted with caution. Second, reliance on self-reported retrospective measures may introduce recall bias. Third, the exclusion of fathers from the study limits insights into potential gender differences in the examined relationships, so caution is advised when generalizing the findings. Fourth, the use of self-reported measures may lead to shared method variance. Finally, while the study's primary focus was on direct relationships and mediating variables, future research could benefit from considering covariates such as maternal age, socioeconomic status, or education level to further enrich the findings and provide additional context.

Practical implication

The findings suggest that identifying pre-mentalizing modes of parental reflective functioning as a mediator emphasizes the importance of integrating reflective practices into therapeutic interventions for families affected by CM. Therapists and practitioners can focus on enhancing parents' abilities to reflect on their own and their child's mental states, which in turn can promote healthier parent–child relationships and improve children's emotional and behavioral well-being.

Conclusion

This study examined the mediation of PRF and PSS in the relationship between maternal CM and children's emotional and behavioral problems among mothers of preschool children in Iran. The findings revealed that maternal CM is not directly related to children's emotional and behavioral issues. Instead, the relationship is indirect, mediated through PM modes, highlighting the importance of these mediating factors in understanding the association between maternal experiences and children's emotional and behavioral problems. While PRFQ-PM showed significant indirect effects on specific subscales of children's emotional and behavioral problems, such as emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer relationship problems, no significant indirect relationship was found for the total emotional-behavioral problems scale through either PRF or PSS. Additionally, PSS did not significantly mediate any of the emotional or behavioral outcomes. However, the relationship between maternal CM and children's emotional and behavioral problems, mediated sequentially by PSS and PRF, was not statistically significant.

Our research supports previous studies that emphasize the role of various mediators in the relationship between maternal CM and children's behavioral outcomes. This study strengthens the literature by identifying PM modes as a critical mediator, indicating that maternal experiences with CM may influence their parenting practices and, consequently, their children's emotions and behavior. The findings also underline the complex interplay of maternal reflective functioning, with implications for interventions targeting mentalization abilities, especially PM modes, to mitigate negative parenting patterns and improve children's emotional and behavioral health.

Data availability

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

References

  1. Stoltenborgh M, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Alink LRA, van IJzendoorn MH. The prevalence of child maltreatment across the globe: review of a series of meta-analyses. Child Abuse Rev. 2015;24(1):37-50.

  2. Yehuda R, Lehrner A. Intergenerational transmission of trauma effects: putative role of epigenetic mechanisms. World Psychiatry. 2018;17(3):243–57.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Rafii F, Alinejad-Naeini M, Peyrovi H. Maternal role attainment in mothers with term neonate: a hybrid concept analysis. Iran J Nurs Midwifery Res. 2020;25(4):304–13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Airikka A, Lahti-Pulkkinen M, Tuovinen S, Heinonen K, Lahti J, Girchenko P, et al. Maternal exposure to childhood maltreatment and mental and behavioral disorders in children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2023;32(12):2463–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Giallo R, Gartland D, Seymour M, Conway L, Mensah F, Skinner L, Fogarty A, Brown S. Maternal childhood abuse and children’s emotional-behavioral difficulties: Intergenerational transmission via birth outcomes and psychosocial health. J Fam Psychol. 2020;34(1):112–21.

  6. Kang NR, Kwack YS, Song JK, Kim MD, Park JH, Kim BN, Moon DS. The impact of maternal adverse childhood experiences on offspring’s internalizing and externalizing problems. Psychiatry Investig. 2021;18(11):1050–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Ma X, Biaggi A, Sacchi C, Lawrence AJ, Chen P-J, Pollard R, et al. Mediators and moderators in the relationship between maternal childhood adversity and children’s emotional and behavioural development: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2022;52(10):1817–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Rijlaarsdam J, Stevens GWJM, Jansen PW, Ringoot AP, Jaddoe VWV, Hofman A, et al. Maternal childhood maltreatment and offspring emotional and behavioral problems: maternal and paternal mechanisms of risk transmission. Child Maltreat. 2014;19(2):67–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Harris M, MacMillan H, Mepham J, Joshi D, Wekerle C, Atkinson L, Gonzalez A. Maternal childhood maltreatment history and child behavior problems: developmental patterns and mediation via maternal depressive symptoms and parenting behavior. Child Maltreat. 2022;28(2):254–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Ogundele MO. Behavioural and emotional disorders in childhood: a brief overview for paediatricians. World J Clin Pediatr. 2018;7(1):9–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Uy JP, Tan AP, Broeckman BBFP, Gluckman PD, Chong YS, Chen H, et al. Effects of maternal childhood trauma on child emotional health: maternal mental health and frontoamygdala pathways. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2023;64(3):426–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Luo S, Chen D, Li C, Lin L, Chen W, Ren Y, et al. Maternal adverse childhood experiences and behavioral problems in preschool offspring: the mediation role of parenting styles. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2023;17(1):95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Weiqiang Y, Ruoyu L, Yongling H, Haiyan H, Chunyan P, Xinzhu W, Yuhui W. Relationship between maternal adverse childhood experiences and preschool children’s emotional and behavioral problems. Chin J School Health. 2022;43(8):1130.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Schore AN. Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Ment Health J: Infancy Early Childhood. 2001;22(1–2):7–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Van der Kolk BA. The body keeps the score: Brain, mind, and body in the healing of trauma. New York, NY: Penguin Books; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Siegel DJ. The developing mind: How relationships and the brain interact to shape who we are. New York: Guilford Publications; 2020.

  17. Schwerdtfeger KL, Larzelere RE, Werner D, Peters C, Oliver M. Intergenerational transmission of trauma: the mediating role of parenting styles on toddlers’ DSM-related symptoms. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma. 2013;22(2):211–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Madigan S, Cyr C, Eirich R, Fearon RMP, Ly A, Rash C, et al. Testing the cycle of maltreatment hypothesis: Meta-analytic evidence of the intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment. Dev Psychopathol. 2019;31(1):23–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Austin A. Association of adverse childhood experiences with life course health and development. N C Med J. 2018;79(2):99–103.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Pop-Jordanova N. Transgenerational transmission of trauma–Matryoshka theory. Acad Med J. 2021;1(1):3–10.

    Google Scholar 

  21. van de Ven MCJ, van den Heuvel MI, Bhogal A, Lewis T, Thomason ME. Impact of maternal childhood trauma on child behavioral problems: the role of child frontal alpha asymmetry. Dev Psychobiol. 2020;62(2):154–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Choi KW, Sikkema KJ, Vythilingum B, Geerts L, Faure SC, Watt MH, et al. Maternal childhood trauma, postpartum depression, and infant outcomes: avoidant affective processing as a potential mechanism. J Affect Disord. 2017;211:107–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Myhre MC, Dyb GA, Wentzel-Larsen T, Grøgaard JB, Thoresen S. Maternal childhood abuse predicts externalizing behaviour in toddlers: a prospective cohort study. Scand J Public Health. 2013;42(3):263–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Stuhrmann LY, Göbel A, Bindt C, Mudra S. Parental reflective functioning and its association with parenting behaviors in infancy and early childhood: a systematic review. Front Psychol. 2022;13: 765312.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Luyten P, Mayes LC, Nijssens L, Fonagy P. The parental reflective functioning questionnaire: development and preliminary validation. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(5): e0176218.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Kelly K, Slade A, Grienenberger JF. Maternal reflective functioning, mother–infant affective communication, and infant attachment: exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behavior in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attach Hum Dev. 2005;7(3):299–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Dehghan Manshadi Z, Fallah A, Chavoshi H. Childhood maltreatment and sense of parenting competence: the mediating role of parental reflective functioning and perceived social support. Child Abuse Negl. 2023;135: 105949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Lieberman AF, Van Horn P. Psychotherapy with infants and young children: Repairing the effects of stress and trauma on early attachment. New York: Guilford Press; 2011.

  29. Thomas V. Does Self-care Moderate the Association between Adverse Childhood Experiences, Trauma Symptoms, and Parental Reflective Functioning? (Master's thesis, East Tennessee State University); 2024.

  30. Fonagy P, Bateman A. The development of borderline personality disorder—a mentalizing model. J Pers Disord. 2008;22(1):4–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Lieberman AF, Ippen CG, Van Horn P. " Don't Hit My Mommy!": a manual for child-parent psychotherapy with young children exposed to violence and other trauma: zero to three. 2015.

  32. van Rensburg EJ, Woolard A, Hill NT, Reid C, Milroy H, Ohan JL, Lin A, Chamberlain C. The effect of childhood maltreatment on adult survivors’ parental reflective function, and attachment of their children: A systematic review. Dev Psychopathol. 2023;36(3):1166–80.

  33. Khoshroo S, Seyed Mousavi PS. Parental reflective functioning as a moderator for the relationship between maternal depression and child internalizing and externalizing problems. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2022;53(6):1319–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kristyn W, Ann MS, Katherine LR, Maria M. Parental reflective functioning moderates the relationship between difficult temperament in infancy and behavior problems in toddlerhood. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2017;63(1):54–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Shalev I, Sharon N, Uzefovsky F, Atzaba-Poria N. Parental guilt and children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior: The moderating role of parental reflective functioning. J Fam Psychol. 2023;37(8):1241–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Nijssens L, Vliegen N, Luyten P. The mediating role of parental reflective functioning in child social–emotional development. J Child Fam Stud. 2020;29(8):2342–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Garon-Bissonnette J, Duguay G, Lemieux R, Dubois-Comtois K, Berthelot N. Maternal childhood abuse and neglect predicts offspring development in early childhood: The roles of reflective functioning and child sex. Child Abuse Negl. 2022;128: 105030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Choi JY. Association between maternal emotional maltreatment history and children’s problem behaviors: The mediating role of mothers’ mentalization and emotion socialization. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2023;93(5):426–35.

  39. Bennett C, Regan D, Dunsmore JC, King G, Westrupp EM. Social and emotional determinants of parental reflective functioning in a multinational sample. J Fam Psychol. 2023;37(6):818–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Shohrati M, Nemattavosi M. The relationship of maternal traumatic childhood experiences and children’s emotional and behavioral problems: the mediating role of reflective functioning. J Psychol Stud. 2023;19(2):39–53.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Riva Crugnola C, Ierardi E, Canevini MP. Reflective functioning, maternal attachment, mind-mindedness, and emotional availability in adolescent and adult mothers at infant 3 months. Attach Hum Dev. 2018;20(1):84–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Wang X. Intergenerational effects of childhood maltreatment: the roles of parents’ emotion regulation and mentalization. Child Abuse Negl. 2022;128: 104940.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Awang MM, Kutty FM, Ahmad AR. Perceived social support and well being: first-year student experience in university. Int Educ Stud. 2014;7(13):261–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Day AL, Livingstone HA. Gender differences in perceptions of stressors and utilization of social support among university students. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement. 2003;35(2):73–83.

  45. Hatch V, Swerbenski H, Gray SA. Family social support buffers the intergenerational association of maternal adverse childhood experiences and preschoolers’ externalizing behavior. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2020;90(4):489–501.

  46. Kong J, Martire LM. Parental childhood maltreatment and the later-life relationship with parents. Psychol Aging. 2019;34(7):900–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Struck N, Krug A, Feldmann M, Yuksel D, Stein F, Schmitt S, et al. Attachment and social support mediate the association between childhood maltreatment and depressive symptoms. J Affect Disord. 2020;273:310–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Nunes C, Martins C, Ayala-Nunes L, Matos F, Costa E, Gonçalves A. Parents’ perceived social support and children’s psychological adjustment. J Soc Work. 2020;21(3):497–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Schiff M, Pat-Horenczyk R, Ziv Y, Brom D. Multiple traumas, maternal depression, mother-child relationship, social support, and young children’s behavioral problems. J Interpers Violence. 2017;36(1–2):892–914.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. León MJ, Olhaberry M. Triadic interactions, parental reflective functioning, and early social-emotional difficulties. Infant Ment Health J: Infancy Early Childhood. 2020;41(4):431–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Shahabi R. Parenting values of Iranian mothers with preschool children. Women Fam Cult-Educ. 2024;19(68):237–55.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Khosravi Z, Cheraghmollaei L, Soleimani M. Parenting values, good parents based on childrenès perspective and ideal children based on parent’s perspective: a qualitative study. J Woman Fam Stud. 2023;11(2):158–90.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Hair J, Hult GTM, Ringle C, Sarstedt M. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) (3rd ed.); New York: Sage publications; 2022.

  54. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the childhood trauma questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(2):169–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Garrusi B, Nakhaee N. Validity and reliability of a persian version of the childhood trauma questionnaire. Psychol Rep. 2009;104(2):509–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(11):1337–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Shahrivar Z, Tehrani-Doost M, Pakbaz B, Rezaie A, Ahmadi F. Normative data and psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ) in an Iranian community sample. J Res Med Sci. 2009;14(2):69–77.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Luyten P, Nijssens L, Fonagy P, Mayes LC. Parental reflective functioning: theory, research, and clinical applications. Psychoanal Study Child. 2017;70(1):174–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Rutherford HJV, Goldberg B, Luyten P, Bridgett DJ, Mayes LC. Parental reflective functioning is associated with tolerance of infant distress but not general distress: evidence for a specific relationship using a simulated baby paradigm. Infant Behav Dev. 2013;36(4):635–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Procidano ME, Heller K. Measures of perceived social support from friends and from family: three validation studies. Am J Community Psychol. 1983;11(1):1–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Evans SE, Steel AL, DiLillo D. Child maltreatment severity and adult trauma symptoms: does perceived social support play a buffering role? Child Abuse Negl. 2013;37(11):934–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Bagherian-Sararoudi R, Hajian A, Ehsan HB, Sarafraz MR, Zimet GD. Psychometric properties of the persian version of the multidimensional scale of perceived social support in iran. Int J Prev Med. 2013;4(11):1277–81.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Lockwood CM, MacKinnon DP, editors. Bootstrapping the standard error of the mediated effect. Proceedings of the 23rd annual meeting of SAS Users Group International. 1998.

  65. Goodhue D, Lewis W, Thompson R. Research note: Does PLS have advantages for small sample size or non-normal data? MIS Q. 2012;36:981–1001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Fong LHN, Law R. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM). Eur J Tour Res. 2013;6:211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Henseler J, Dijkstra TK, Sarstedt M, Ringle CM, Diamantopoulos A, Straub DW, et al. Common Beliefs and Reality About PLS: Comments on Rönkkö and Evermann (2013). Organ Res Methods. 2014;17(2):182–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Malhotra NK. Marketing research: an applied prientation. United Kingdom: Pearson; 2020.

  69. Hair Jr JF, Hult GTM, Ringle CM, Sarstedt M, Danks NP, Ray S, et al. An introduction to structural equation modeling. Partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) using R: a workbook. 2021:1-29.

  70. Gold AH, Malhotra A, Segars AH. Knowledge Management: An Organizational Capabilities Perspective. J Manag Inf Syst. 2001;18(1):185–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Su Y, D’Arcy C, Meng X. Intergenerational effect of maternal childhood maltreatment on next generation’s vulnerability to psychopathology: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2022;23(1):152–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Greene CA, Haisley L, Wallace C, Ford JD. Intergenerational effects of childhood maltreatment: a systematic review of the parenting practices of adult survivors of childhood abuse, neglect, and violence. Clin Psychol Rev. 2020;80: 101891.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Clemens V, Wernecke D, Fegert J, Jon G, Rothenbacher D, Braig S. Maternal child maltreatment and trajectories of offspring behavioural and emotional difficulties from age 4 to 7 years – results from a prospective birth cohort study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2024;34:1039–50.

  74. Özsoy F, Taşcı İ. Defense mechanisms, dissociation, alexithymia and childhood traumas in chronic migraine patients. J Rational-Emot Cognitive-Behav Ther. 2021;39(1):101–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Vaillant GE. Ego mechanisms of defense: a guide for clinicans and researchers. New York: Am J Psychiatry. 1992.

  76. McLaughlin KA, Lambert HK. Child trauma exposure and psychopathology: mechanisms of risk and resilience. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;14:29–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Yang L, Huang M. Childhood maltreatment and mentalizing capacity: a meta-analysis. Child Abuse Negl. 2024;149: 106623.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Gershy N, Gray SAO. Parental emotion regulation and mentalization in families of children with ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2020;24(14):2084–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Nabavi RT. Bandura’s social learning theory & social cognitive learning theory. Theory Dev Psychol. 2012;1(1):1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Strathearn L. Borderline personality disorder, mentalization, and the neurobiology of attachment. Infant Ment Health J. 2011;32(1):47–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Euler S, Wrege J, Busmann M, Lindenmeyer HJ, Sollberger D, Lang UE, et al. Exclusion-proneness in borderline personality disorder inpatients impairs alliance in mentalization-based group therapy. Front Psychol. 2018;9:824.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Euler S, Nolte T, Constantinou M, Griem J, Montague PR, Fonagy P, et al. Interpersonal problems in borderline personality disorder: associations with mentalizing, emotion regulation, and impulsiveness. J Pers Disord. 2021;35(2):177–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Álvarez N, Lázaro MH, Gordo L, Elejalde LI, Pampliega AM. Maternal mentalization and child emotion regulation: A comparison of different phases of early childhood. Infant Behav Dev. 2022;66: 101681.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Pazzagli C, Delvecchio E, Raspa V, Mazzeschi C, Luyten P. The parental reflective functioning questionnaire in mothers and fathers of school-aged children. J Child Family Stud. 2018;27:80–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Milan S, Carlone C, Printz D, Perez SD. Understanding children’s emotions: differences in mothers with a history of childhood maltreatment. Child Maltreat. 2020;27(1):33–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Raval VV, Li X, Deo N, Hu J. Reports of maternal socialization goals, emotion socialization behaviors, and child functioning in China and India. J Fam Psychol. 2018;32(1):81–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Shahaeian A, Nielsen M, Peterson CC, Slaughter V. Iranian mothers’ disciplinary strategies and theory of mind in children: A focus on belief understanding. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2014;45(7):1110–23.

  88. Naik V, Trivedi G, Trivedi RG. Navigating generational patterns: how childhood trauma shapes authoritative parenting practices in Indian families. Int J Indian Psychȯl. 2024;12(2):4179–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. O’Shields J, Mowbray O, Cooper Z. The effects of childhood maltreatment on social support, inflammation, and depressive symptoms in adulthood. Soc Sci Med. 2024;340:116481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Saluja GK, Khanam A. Impact of childhood trauma on perceived social support. Int J Res Publ Rev. 2023;4(6):1176–80.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to grateful all parents who agreed to participate in this investigation in Shiraz, Iran, and Agah psychology Clinic.

Funding

No funding was obtained.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Z.D.M. and M.R.S. was involved in the study design, data collection, analysis, and writing of the article. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Zobeydeh Dehghan Manshadi or Mehdi Reza Sarafraz.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study adhered to the guidelines set forth in the Declaration of Helsinki, was approved by the ethical committee at Shiraz University (IR.US.PSYEDU.REC.1403.031). Informed written consent was obtained from the mothers of all participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dehghan Manshadi, Z., Sarafraz, M.R. Relationship of maternal childhood maltreatment and children’s emotional-behavioral problems: parental reflection functioning and social support’s role. BMC Psychol 13, 318 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s40359-025-02627-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s40359-025-02627-x

Keywords