Your privacy, your choice

We use essential cookies to make sure the site can function. We also use optional cookies for advertising, personalisation of content, usage analysis, and social media.

By accepting optional cookies, you consent to the processing of your personal data - including transfers to third parties. Some third parties are outside of the European Economic Area, with varying standards of data protection.

See our privacy policy for more information on the use of your personal data.

for further information and to change your choices.

Skip to main content

The impact of the student perception of teacher talk on student language learning attainment

Abstract

While prior studies provide valuable insights into the role of teacher talk and its impact on learning outcomes, there remains a need for further research on how contextual factors may interact with student perceptions to influence language learning. This study explores how students’ perceptions of teacher talk influence their English language learning outcomes, focusing on the mediating roles of student enjoyment and emotional exhaustion, as well as the moderating role of self-efficacy. The research involved a cross-sectional survey with 357 university students, and the data were analyzed using structural equation modeling (SEM). Findings affirm that mindful perceptions of teacher talk enhance students’ enjoyment level thus the achievement of better English language learning. On the other hand, negative perceptions result in a condition known as the emotional exhaustion which has negative influence on learning experience. Contrasting with emotional exhaustion, student self-efficacy was established to moderate interaction with enjoyment and increase the impact of advantage on language attainment. Such results emphasize the necessity of constructing positive and stimulating teacher-student communication as well as the enhancement of student’s perceived self-efficacy.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

The ability to convey oneself in the English language is highly valued in the contemporary world, where learners from diverse backgrounds often interact in academic, professional, and personal domains [1, 2]. In formal educational settings, the central role of teachers in facilitating and guiding language use is undeniable [3]. Among the myriad factors influencing second language acquisition, teacher talk, defined as the language teachers use during instruction, has emerged as a particularly salient determinant of students’ performance [4]. Previous studies suggest that the quality and quantity of teacher talk can shape learners’ cognitive and affective engagement [5], and that students’ reception of this talk, how comprehensible or meaningful they perceive it, strongly influences motivation and active participation [5, 6]. Conversely, negative perceptions, such as boredom or frustration, may lead to disengagement and hinder language development [7, 8].

Despite the evident significance of student perception of teacher talk, prior research has not fully explored how and why these perceptions translate into differential learning outcomes in second language contexts. For example, scholars have noted that it is unclear how teacher talk translates into concrete student learning outcomes [5, 9]. As such, analyzing student perception of teacher talk as a concept, may help its role in determining different aspects of language learning may be profound [10]. Thus, studying the factors which can either enhance or reduce the correlation between the specified perception and interest in the outcome of language learning could be helpful in improving educational practices, as well as in fine-tuning learning arrangements.

Moreover, there remains a gap in understanding the mediating and moderating mechanisms that underpin this relationship. Scholars have highlighted two key affective processes, student enjoyment and emotional exhaustion, as potentially critical mediators [11, 12]. Enjoyment, characterized by the joy and personal satisfaction derived from engagement in learning tasks, consistently predicts academic performance across various domains, including language learning [13, 14]. Conversely, emotional exhaustion, feelings of fatigue and withdrawal, can detract from a student’s capacity to manage learning efforts and persist in challenging tasks [15, 16]. The knowledge of the processes which connect student perception of teacher talk and English language learning outcomes is crucial in designing instructors’ strategies that foster positive learning and avoid negative occurrences.

In addition, the moderating variable of student self-efficacy cannot be left out in this relationship as well. Self-efficacy according to Bandura [17] and Ali, Wang [18], entails the confidence of people in their ability to perform specific task or actions required for accomplishing specified levels of performances. Various aspects of the model can be best defined by illustrating its theoretical constructs in the context of students’ perceptions of their own English language learning capacities and how they affect students’ motivation, participation, persistence, and learning [19]. Hence, understanding how student self-efficacy can moderate the relationships between the student perception of teacher talk and English language learning attainment, one can extend some more light on the conditions that promote extraverted teacher talk for the improvement of language proficiency of students.

To address above gaps in the literature, this uses following research question to develops a model (Fig. 1) to investigate how students’ perceptions of teacher talk influence English language attainment through the mediating roles of student enjoyment and emotional exhaustion, while also examining whether self-efficacy strengthens or weakens these pathways.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Proposed model

  1. 1.

    How do students’ perceptions of teacher talk influence their English language learning outcomes, particularly within technology-mediated instructional contexts?

  2. 2.

    To what extent do students’ affective states, enjoyment and emotional exhaustion, mediate the relationship between perceived teacher talk and language-learning performance?

  3. 3.

    Does self-efficacy moderate these mediated pathways?

By integrating these affective and psychological constructs into a unified framework, this study aims to provide a more nuanced understanding of the conditions under which teacher talk is most beneficial—or detrimental—for second language learners. Ultimately, these insights can guide educators in tailoring their instructional language and scaffolding strategies to optimize student motivation, reduce burnout, and enhance overall language proficiency.

Literature review and hypothesis development

Student perception of teacher talk and english language learning attainment relationship

This paper shows that student attitude towards teacher talk is a key factor that affect students’ performance, participation, and motivation in their learning of English as a foreign language. Teacher talk therefore refers to the total of all the verbal responses from the teacher during learning intervention that entails; teacher explanation, teacher feedback, and interactional discourse [20, 21]. Teacher talk refers to the extent, quality, and frequency of teacher’s speaking in the classroom, which is essential as it offers the major source of input to learn second language by students [22]. Nonetheless, it is equally important to realize that the affluent use of language that we have been discussing as a part of teacher talk is not just a question of the quality of language being employed. Observations of teacher-student communication, specifically, students’ attitudes of perceiving teacher talk as comprehensible, reassuring, and inviting, lead to increased participation, which in turn results in greater language learning [23]. On the other hand, negative perceptions like perceiving teacher talk as difficult and complex may have negative contribution towards student motivation and advancement in English language learning [24].

Intuitively, existing studies have established a correlation between the type of teacher talk from the students’ perspective and their language learning progress [5, 10, 25]. For example, Mercer and Dörnyei [25] argue that when students are able to understand teacher talk and consider the talk to be scaffolded, students feel competent and autonomous in the learning process, which are features of intrinsic motivation according to self-determination theory by Deci and Ryan [26]. This feeling of efficiency and independency results in better cognitive absorption and longer time spent on accomplishing language acquisition goals, which ultimately leads to a higher level of English language attainment [2, 27]. Research has also indicated that teacher talk in terms of use of open-ended questions, urges to expand and generate more language use, and generic prompts that allow students to take over the conversation, enhances students’ language development by involving them actively in language use and critical thinking [28, 29]. These interactional properties of teacher talk are not only beneficial in offering complex forms of input but also in contributing to the development of a stimulating social context that enables learners to engage in repeated acts of target language use.

The above findings are also supported by other studies that establish the connection between student perceptions of teacher talk and English language learning based on interactive and student-oriented teacher talk. Kim and Elder [30], identified that in a context where teacher talk is used in conjunction with interactive techniques such as elicitation, scaffolding, and feedback, higher levels of learning and language attainment were achieved. If the students are able to come to the conclusion that the teacher talk is not a one way form of communication but actually engages with student needs, then they will be encouraged to learn through the more meaningful form of communication which is language acquisition [31, 32]. This is in concord with the hypothesis by Long [33], which suggests that language learner gains from interactional modifications in teacher talk that makes input understandable and that which offers the learner an avenue to respond to. Thus, it is significant for the students’ linguistic acquisition and achievement that these aspects of the teacher’s discourse should be valued by the students.

Mediating roles of student enjoyment student emotional exhaustion

The mediating influence of a variable, in this case, student enjoyment is critical in the interdependence between teacher talk and the performance of students in English language learning as their second language. Enjoyment, defined as affective states of pleasure, interest and intrinsic motivation in the context of learning activities constitutes one of the affective variables which can positively impact cognition and academic achievement [14]. Fredricks, Blumenfeld [34], argue that when student’s perceive teacher’s talk as relevant and encouraging, this lead to their enjoyment thus creating increased commitment towards learning efforts. It also entails improved mastery and usage of English language amongst the beneficiaries owing to the enhanced engagement. Besides, enjoyment has a self-reinforcing effect; increased interest in the learning activities leads to more effort and perseverance [35], which forms the basis of becoming a second-language user [36]. Therefore, teacher talk, being perceived enjoyable mediates the level of learning and development in realizing higher level of language proficiency.

On the other hand, the level of the student burnout could explain the degree of the detrimental effects of the excessive teacher talk on the English language learning achievements [37]. A more specific aspect of burnout that needs to be surveyed is the measure of emotional exhaustion, defined as a depletion of emotional resources owing to prolonged stress and strain [11]. Thus, when in educational context the teacher’s discourse can may decrease the feelings of emotional distress among learners [15]. Scholars have found that social contexts shape emotional exhaustion [38,39,40]. Such a state of exhaustion can be said to have a negative impact on the students’ learning ability in terms of their ability to learn and assimilate new information, a crucial aspect in language learning [41]. In a separate study conducted by Zhang and Sapp [42], it was also evident that high levels of emotional exhaustion are inversely proportional to the levels of the students’ academic achievement because most of the exhausted learners rarely attend classes or submit assignments. Thus, by serving as a mediator variable, emotional exhaustion suggests that appropriate course mechanics may alleviate the benefits of teacher talk, an indication of a need for moderation and a favorable classroom climate.

Hypotheses 1: Student enjoyment mediates the relationship between student perception of teacher talk and student English language learning attainment.

Hypotheses 2: Student emotional exhaustion mediates the relationship between student perception of teacher talk and student English language learning attainment.

Moderating role of student self-efficacy

Another important research question is whether and how student self-efficacy moderates the impact of teacher talk on second language learning and more specifically, English language learning [43,44,45]. Self-efficacies, which refers to students’ expectations regarding their capabilities for learning and achieving academically [17], can have a strong impact on the form through which students comprehend as well as their reaction towards teacher’s discourse [46]. Self-efficacy has been found to influence students’ approach towards tasks as depicted by the fact that highly efficacious students tend to believe that they can accomplish difficult tasks and are more likely to persevere when faced with obstacles [19]. Applying the framework of self-efficacy in language acquisition context, it can be suggested that high level of self-efficacy can support the efficiency of teacher talk by increasing students’ confidence in the future learning process and in the ability to understand and use the target language. For example, Graham [47], notes that self-efficacious students are more capable in using the teachers’ feedback as well as instructional talk to enhance language development, in part because they will be more inclined and persistent in their practice unlike others who give up easily. Consequently, self-efficacy has a multiplicative effect whereby it enhances the contribution of teacher talk towards the learners’ language achievement.

However, the students with low self-efficacy may have different perceptions towards teacher talk and as such the results may be less effective. Low self-efficacy as found by Schunk and Pajares [48], causes anxiety among the students and makes them doubt their capability to do language tasks hence developing fear and reluctance not to engage in the learning process. Such students may find it difficult to engage with places where they experience teacher talk which they consider to be hard or which they consider in some way taxing and will therefore refrain from fully participating in classroom discussions or engaging in practice that involves the use of language. This in turn can lead to a vicious cycle by which low self efficacy reduces the effects of teacher talk lowering in turn the linguistic competency. In their study of the inter related mechanisms of selves, Hassankhani, Mohajjel Aghdam [49], also noted that self-efficacy affected not only motivation but also moderated the consequences between instructional practice and performance, these findings imply that enhancing self-efficacy could help optimize the outcomes of teacher talk in language learning context.

In addition, the analysis reveals that self-efficacy can operate as a moderator of the relationship between instructional practices and student achievement [50], thus implying potential instructional approaches to be used when teaching to a heterogeneous class of students. The studies have focused on how students with different levels of self-efficacy study during teachers’ talk time and found that there is a difference in students’ study behavior on the basis of their self-efficacy [51, 52]. For instance, Tschannen-Moran and Hoy [53], offer a study that indicates how teachers can leverage self-efficacies through mastery experiences, positive verbal persuasion and coaching or role modeling in an effort to enhance the learning environment. It is also explored by Goddard, Tschannen-Moran [54], that in a practical activity, this could include offering gradual increases in difficulty to help build up student’s confidence, using affirming language and feedback, and modelling the correct use of language. Moreover, it has been reported by Kim and Baylor [55], that presence of adaptive learning/teaching approaches that incorporates learners’ self-efficacy levels contributes towards better learning engagement and achievement. Furthermore, the integration of these strategies will help offset the drawbacks involving low self-efficacy and enhance the advantages of teachers’ instructional talk so as to achieve better English language mastery for students with different learning characteristics.

Hypothesis 3

Student self-efficacy moderates the indirect effect of student perception of teacher talk and student English language learning attainment via student enjoyment such that the indirect is more positive at a high level of student self-efficacy than at low level of student self-efficacy.

Hypothesis 4

Student self-efficacy moderates the indirect effect of student perception of teacher talk and student English language learning attainment via student emotional exhaustion such that the indirect is less negative at a high level of student self-efficacy than at low level of student self-efficacy.

The present study

The present study uses an elaborate model that captures multiple variables to determine the relationship between student’s perception of teacher’s talk and English language learning attainment with mediating and moderating effects. Teacher talk specifically refers to the student perception of teachers’ verbal input, as defined by Ellis [20], as a key that would unlock the impact of student perceptions of teacher talk on the English language learning outcomes. This relationship is hypothesized to be mediated by two critical affective variables: students’ enjoyment and students’ emotional exhaustion. Enjoyment, defined as positive affect and intrinsic motivation during learning activities as described by Pekrun, Goetz [8], will help increase cognitive investment and, on this ground, impact English language learning attainment. Emotional exhaustion, one of the sub dimension of burnout emphasizing on the feeling of fatigue and decrease in engagement Maslach, Schaufeli [11], is expected to reduce students’ capacity to capture and store new linguistic information, thereby negatively influencing second language acquisition. Apart from these mediating variables, there is another variable, which is student self-efficacy as the moderation variable in our proposed model (Figure: 1). Self-efficacy which refers to the level of confidence learners hold to persevere in language learning tasks [17], is assumed to determine how students interpret and/or respond to teacher talk. By hypothesizing a positive, moderational relationship between self-efficacy and the teacher explanation variable, it is assumed that the use of supportive teacher talk will be more beneficial and effective for self-efficacious students than less self-efficacious students because these kinds of students are more capable to work hard on challenging tasks. On the other hand, low self-efficacy might enhance the detrimental effects of critical or unsupportive teacher talk which on pupils’ level results in increased anxiety and subsequent avoidance of learning [56]. Through the incorporation of these mediating and moderating variables into the flowchart, a key aspect of the design of this proposed model is to depict the temporal and contextual complexity of language learning to give a comprehensive explanation of the factors that determine English language learning. This is a broad-spectrum strategy intended to be beneficial for educators, presenting the need to embrace both emotions and thought processes when designing instruction methods [25, 57].

The present study mainly contributes to the literature by answering the following research questions: (1) to what extent is student-perceived teacher talk an influential factor within attaining better English language learning through the mediating variable of student enjoyment? This notion is in line with a study by Pekrun, Goetz [14], whereas Dewaele and MacIntyre [36], also found out that, when the students considered teacher talk as comprehensible, motivating, and affiliative, their enjoyment and self-generated interest would be increased, thereby promoting foreign language learning. Intrinsic motivation mechanisms indicate enjoyment and suggests that positive affective experiences play a key function in enhancing detailed cognitive processing and higher persistence in undertaking language learning activities [34]. (2) To what extent student perceived teacher talk affects English language learning attainment through the mediator variable, namely student emotional exhaustion. Thus, by the end of a term, victims of academic stress may experience a state when they feel emotionally detached from their course of study and suffering from burnout and therefore, when handling a new language, the students would be impaired in their ability to retrieve information as their cognitive processes have been compromised. When the teacher talk is seen negatively it is likely to increase the feelings of emotional exhaustion and hence discourage language learning [42]. (3) To what extent does student self-efficacy have a moderation effect on the student’s enjoyment and emotional exhaustion belief towards English language learning achievement? It is important to note that self-efficacy refers to students’ beliefs with regard to their capabilities regarding the accomplishment of given learning tasks in language, such that enjoyment can enhance the former while minimizing the latter in a given setting [19]. Self-efficacy improves students’ stability and enthusiasm since it helps shield one from adverse stress impacts and consistently supports more effective academic outcomes [48].

Methods

Data collection procedures

The participants of the study consisted of 357 Chinese learners of English drawn from a school district in a Chinese city through convenience sampling. A school district in a Chinese city may have been chosen because it is readily accessible to the researchers. A researchers already be in the region and have existing relationships with the schools, making it easier to obtain permission and recruit participants. In addition, author have chosen a district that has a higher concentration of schools with English language programs, increasing the likelihood of gathering rich, relevant data for the study Author has used convenience sampling method for this study for several reasons. First, convenience sampling is cost-effective because it saves time and resources. Second, convenience sampling helps researchers make use of whatever participants are available. For example, in small, localized studies or when studying specific groups of individuals who are readily accessible (e.g., university students for campus studies), it may be the most feasible option. Prior scholars also observed that convenience sampling can be particularly useful for testing hypotheses or conducting pilot studies before larger-scale, more rigorous sampling methods are employed. In Structural Equation Modeling (SEM), determining an appropriate sample size is essential for conducting a reliable and efficient analysis. To ensure that the sample size was sufficient, we used statistical power analysis to calculate the required number of participants, utilizing the G*Power software. Statistical power is a critical measure that indicates the likelihood of detecting an effect if one truly exists, and it is important for ensuring that the results are not due to random chance. For this study, statistical power of 0.95, which exceeds the commonly recommended threshold of 0.80, which provides confidence in the robustness of the results [58, 59]. By surpassing this threshold, the sample size used in this study is more than adequate for SEM analysis, reducing the risk of Type II errors and increasing the reliability of the findings. As a result, the sample size employed in this study is deemed sufficient to perform SEM analysis effectively, ensuring the statistical rigor and accuracy of the model estimation. The participants were university students in their first-year English preparatory program to enable them do majors that utilize English language as their medium of instruction. The course followed the guidelines of the Common European Framework of Reference (CEFR) for languages, particularly, it can be attributed to the Learning, Teaching, and Assessment, mentioned by West, Güven [60]. The CEFR is intended for use in describing, comparing and coordinating the provision of language education across Europe and divides the ability to use a language into six broad levels (A1-C2). From 357, there were 196 males and 161 females with an average age of 31 to 40 years. The demographic characteristics of participants are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic information of the samples

Instruments

Student perception of teacher talk

Perceived teacher talk was measured by six items specifically developed for this study, stemming from prior research on academically productive types of teacher talk [5, 61,62,63]. Consequently, while many studies over the years have captured the characteristics of classroom discourse from the teacher’s vantage point, no prior research has targeted the perceptions of students as the main protagonists of the analysis. This study however developed and pilot tested the student-perceived teacher classroom talk scale to assess the perceptions that students have on their English language teacher’s classroom talk. The scale aligns with four key goals for fostering classroom discussions: It assists in the following ways: (1) sharing of ideas among the students, (2) active listening amongst the students, (3) builds the students’ critical thinking skills, and (4) cooperative thinking [61, 62]. Students were also urged to indicate the frequency with which their English teachers integrated academic talk based on six items (e.g., “My English language teacher makes us explain the ideas in the classroom,” “My English language teacher permits adequate time for responding to questions in the classroom,” “My English language teacher makes us assess one another’s ideas in the classroom”). The responses were collected using a four-point Likert scale where 1 represented rarely, and 5 represented frequently. A higher value for the sum of scores was an increase of the academically productive talk according to the students’ perception.

Students’ emotions in class

To assess the students’ self-reported emotions during the class, we created a scale borrowed from the class-related part of the AEQ [8]. To measure the extent to which students find attending English language class enjoyable, the following four items were used (e.g., “I often look forward to my English language class” and “I enjoy going for my English language class”). It was on a Likert scale with response choices running from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). A higher score on the subscale were suggested to result in greater enjoyment.

The level of student emotional exhaustion was assessed with the help of Maslach Burnout Inventory-Student Survey (MBI-SS) [11], and respectively the subscale of emotional exhaustion. Based on the Maslach Burnout Inventory, MBI-SS we used to assess burnout phenomena in academic context, the signs of which are fatigue, and depletion of emotional reserves. This is a four-item subscale that that has been used in many studies in education setting and is known for its ability to ascertain reliably the level of students’ emotional exhaustion [64]. Some of the sample items that were used: “I feel emotionally drained by my studies”; “I feel burned out from my studies.” The Likert scale used for measuring these variables ranged from 1 (never) to 5 (always). The use of the MBI-SS in our study enabled the measurement of levels of students’ emotional exhaustion as influenced by perceived teacher talk and their resultant.

Questionnaire of self-efficacy in learning a foreign Language (QSLL)

The QSLL is an 11-items structured scale applicable to gauge the participants’ perceived self-efficacy towards the learning of English as foreign language [65]. This instrument comprises a single overarching factor representing overall self-efficacy in English as a second language, as well as two specific group factors: Reading/Listening self-efficacy and Speaking /Writing self-efficacy. The questionnaire items are based on the Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: Language learning, language teaching, language assessment [66], which is the official document defining the reference levels for communicating in a foreign language. An example item for listening is, “I can watch and understand English films and TV series without English/Chinese subtitles.” The responses were on a Likert scale; 1– strongly disagree, 5- strongly agree. The scores were generally derived from the scale and the evaluation criteria were used to calculate a mean score with the higher score relating to higher self-efficacy.

English language attainment

Learner performance was measured in terms of participants’ average English examination result obtained at the end of the English preparatory program. These scores were self- generated by the universities as a composite of bookmark quizzes, mid-semester tests, and semester-end tests. One of the most significant decisions was the choice of the exam structure which consisted in adopting the CEFR as the common framework for the examination. Read, write, listen, and the speak skills were tested, and the scores obtained by learners were summed to give a total score. The maximum that could have been scored in this project was 100%. These examinations were developed and conducted by an independent testing office in each university, mainly the English as foreign language teachers were involved in the conception, writing, and execution of the exams during the academic year. The examinations were internally double marked according to guidelines from the testing offices. The two independent markers separately assigned grades to each student’s portfolio, but in case of differences, the markers discussed the matter with a moderator with the common agreement on the final grade.

Data analysis and results

Analytical strategies

This study was conducted in compliance with the recommendations highlighted by Kumar, Upadhyay [67], and the six stages of data analysis were included in this study are enumerated as follows: First, the missing data, outliers, and multicollinearity were examined by the author. To eliminate the possibility of common method variance, different tests were conducted in the second stage. In the third phase, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted for all the measures to establish the fit indices, internal consistency, and validity coefficients of the study. The fourth stage involved assessing the model fit values of the research model as compared to the other models. Based on the guidelines of Prügl and Spitzley [68], SEM analysis was used on AMOS in the fifth stage to assess the direct effects of the variables, whilst the control variables were regressed on English language learning on English language learning attainment. Furthermore, Hayes [69] PROCESS Model 4 macro was used to establish mediation effects of student enjoyment and student emotional exhaustion. Finally, the moderated mediation analysis was conducted using Hayes [69] PROCESS Model 7. Whereas for Model 4 and model 7, a simple random sample of size B = 5000 was chosen and using 95% level of confidence interval in order to increase the accuracy of the bootstrap estimation [69].

Measurement model

In order to assess the reliability, validity of all the constructs and, thus confirm the quality of the measurement model we conducted reliability analysis, convergent validity and discriminant validity test. For convergent validity, scholars proposed that loading scores should be at least 0.60, while loading values higher than 0. 70 is perfect for the analysis of hypothesis [70]. Further, to assess the internal consistency and reliability of the measures, the CR and CA coefficients were calculated. Research results revealed that both coefficients, namely CR and CA were above 0. 70, as shown in Table 2, which indicates an acceptable level of reliability [70]. Moreover, as presented in Table 2, all the constructs possessed the AVE values more than 0.50 [58], indicating an adequate level of convergent validity. Therefore, the results suggested that the composite reliability values reached the criterion for internal consistency and reliability. Consequently, it was found that all the constructs of our measurement model had acceptable levels of reliability and convergent validity.

Table 2 Results of confirmatory factor analysis

Furthermore, discriminant validity of all the measurement items was confirmed through a multi-method approach. Initially, we scrutinized the cross-loadings of the items of all constructs, as detailed in Table 3. From Table 3, it is apparent that all instruments exhibit loadings greater than 0.60, with substantial loadings in their designated positions and minimal loadings in other columns [71]. Secondly, we analyzed the square root of the AVE of each construct that was depicted in Table 4 against the correlation coefficients between the constructs. From these comparisons, it becomes evident that all the AVE square roots exceed the correlation coefficients as proposed by [70]. Finally, we employed CFA model fit approaches to evaluate the discriminant validity of the research model in the current research [72]. Table 5 provides a comparison of all CFA models demonstrates that all models’ χ2 values exceed the recommended thresholds, indicating significant differences among the models. Furthermore, all CFI differences were above the cutoff value of 0.002. Consequently, discriminant validity is confirmed through both the χ2 difference and CFI difference criteria.

Table 3 Cross-loading
Table 4 Means, standard deviation, and correlations
Table 5 CFA approach displaying model comparison outcomes

Assessment of bias (CMB)/ multicollinearity

Due to the cross-sectional survey data limitations like multicollinearity and CMB, we performed several analysis in the present study. Firstly, we conducted a VIF test to diagnose the level of multicollinearity that may exists in the dataset. The VIF scores of all the principal constructs observed (1 to 1.213), indicating that there is no possibility of multicollinearity in this research study. Additionally, the extent of multicollinearity was evaluated using pairwise squared correlation coefficients. As presented in the correlation matrix (Table 4), the squared correlations of constructs are below the recommended value of 0.80, further confirming that multicollinearity is not a concern in this research [73].

Further, to mitigate potential of CMB issues, we implemented several robust techniques as suggested by [74]. To address this concern, several statistical techniques were employed alongside specific measures implemented during the initial stages of the study [74, 75]. In terms of procedural safeguards, beyond the invitation letter, we made efforts to ensure that participants were given sufficient time and space to complete the survey, thus reducing the chance of rushed or incomplete responses. We also included a statement that clarified participants’ rights, including the protection of their personal data and assurance of anonymity. This helped build trust, further reducing the potential for social desirability bias or self-report biases that might arise from participants feeling pressured to respond in a socially acceptable manner. We also emphasized confidentiality and highlighted that their individual responses would remain anonymous and would be aggregated for analysis. This confidentiality assurance aimed to minimize social desirability biases, where participants might otherwise feel compelled to provide responses they believe would be viewed more favorably. For statistical analysis, first, we scrutinized the correlations among all constructs in Table 4. None of the correlation values surpassed 0.90, with the highest being 0.480, indicating the absence of significant CMB [71]. Second, we analyzed the full data set using Harman’s single factor test through exploratory factor analysis. The first factor accounted for only 14.95% of the variance, which is below the 50% threshold. Finally, we compared the fit of the one-factor model to that of the measurement model, as detailed in Table 5. The fit indices for the one-factor model (χ2 = 10776.28 df = 1313, CFI = 0.430, TLI = 0.402, IFI = 0.432, REMSA = 0.135, SRMR = 0.186) were significantly inferior to those of the measurement model (χ2 = 2817.237, df = 1277, CFI = 0.906, TLI = 0.898, IFI = 0.906, REMSA = 0.055, SRMR = 0.056) [76]. Given the assessments made above as well as the statistical test results, it is possible to conclude that CMB does not pose a threat to the research model employed in this study.

Confirmatory factor analysis

The presented findings of Table 5 indicated the best fit indices for five factor measurement model of the values (χ2 = 2817.237, df = 1277, CFI = 0.906, TLI = 0.898, IFI = 0.906, REMSA = 0.055, SRMR = 0.056) are significantly better than other models. According to Table 5 the value of fit for five-factor model is reasonable when the sample size is above 150 as suggested by [77].

Hypothesis testing

With reference to the measurement model’s validity, the formulated hypotheses are examined through a SEM run in AMOS 21. 0 [58]. Furthermore, prior to testing the mediating hypothesis, SEM was used to determine the path among the constructs as shown in Fig. 2. The outcomes have revealed that student perception of teacher talk has a positive connection with student enjoyment (B = 0.301; t = 6.835; p < 0.001), and have a negative effect with student emotional exhaustion (B = -0.106; t = -2.298; p < 0.01). Student enjoyment (B = 0.125; t = 8.569; p < 0.001), and student emotional exhaustion (B = -0.162; t = -7.677; p < 0.001) are also significantly related to English language learning attainment. Furthermore, the findings showed that all the control variables were not significantly associated with English language learning attainment expect age.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Structural model (Sex = -0.013, Age = 0.077***, Education Level = 0.017). Note: *p < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Control Variables: Gender, Age, Education level are insignificant

Mediation analysis

We also proposed the mediating effects of student enjoyment and student emotional exhaustion. To test these effects, we adopted the bootstrap sampling method (bootstrap sample size = 5000) recommended by MacKinnon, Lockwood [78] to generate asymmetric confidence intervals (CIs) for indirect relationships. Unlike traditional methods such as the Sobel test, the bootstrap CI approach provides a more accurate estimation by producing asymmetric CIs for indirect relationships based on the respective distributions of the two regression coefficients that comprise the product term [78]. Table 6 presents the results of the mediating effects of student enjoyment and student emotional exhaustion. Student enjoyment mediated the relationship between student perception of teacher talk and English language learning attainment, as the CI (LLCI = 0.016; ULCI = 0.056) did not include zero, thus supporting H1. Similarly, student emotional exhaustion mediated the relationship between student perception of teacher talk and English language learning attainment, as the CI (LLCI = 0.006; ULCI = 0.0355) did not include zero, thus supporting H2.

Table 6 Mediation analysis

Moderation analysis

Based on the research model, we incorporated interaction terms to test the moderating effect of student self-efficacy. The results, depicted in Fig. 2, demonstrate that moderation effect of student self-efficacy. The analysis confirmed that the interaction term between student enjoyment and English language learning attainment moderated by student self-efficacy was not significant (B = -0.030; t = 0.634; p > 0.05), interaction term is insignificant. In addition, the interaction term between student emotional exhaustion and English language learning attainment (B = 0.075; t= -2.229; p < 0.05), is significant.

Furthermore, in order to illustrate the pattern for the moderating role of student self-efficacy, we used a graphical technique as recommended by Aiken, West [79]. In addition, Fig. 3 illustrates that the relationship between student emotional exhaustion and English language learning attainment can be weakened at both high (+ 1SD) and low (-1SD) levels of student self-efficacy.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Moderating role of student self-efficacy in the relationship between student emotional exhaustion and english language learning attainment

Moderated mediation model

We tested the moderated mediation model using SPSS process macro for model 7, as recommended by Hayes [69] for testing the moderated mediation. Table 7 presents the bootstrap confidence intervals (CIs) for the indirect effect of student perception of teacher talk on English language learning attainment through student enjoyment at different levels of student self-efficacy: one standard deviation above the mean (LLC = 0.017, ULC = 0.071), at the mean (LLC = 0.016, ULC = 0.057), and one standard deviation below the mean (LLC = 0.010, ULC = 0.054). The hypothesized mediating indirect effect of student perception of teacher talk on English language learning attainment through student enjoyment was found to be significant because the CIs were not included zero, providing the support for H4. Similarly, Table 7 indicates the indirect effect of student perception of teacher talk on English language learning attainment through Student emotional exhaustion at different levels of student self-efficacy for one standard deviation above the mean (LLC = 0. 008, ULC = 0.053), at the mean (LLC = 0.048, ULC = 0.033), and one standard deviation below the mean (LLC = -0. 020, ULC = 0.033), included zero, H5 is rejected.

Table 7 Moderated mediation analysis

Discussion

Implications for theory

The findings of this study extend our theoretical understanding of how students’ perceptions of teacher talk shape their emotional experiences and subsequent language learning outcomes. Specifically, the results suggest that positively perceived teacher talk, characterized by clarity, appeal, and non-threatening delivery, can cultivate enjoyment, which in turn promotes deeper engagement and improved language comprehension and mastery [36, 80]. This positive affective response aligns with the existing theory and research [31, 34], which emphasizes how positive emotions expand cognitive and behavioral repertoires, ultimately fostering more persistent effort and better academic performance [40]. Moreover, the study shows that teacher talk decreases emotional exhaustion, an outcome consistent with the notion that stress depletes cognitive and emotional resources needed for learning [81]. In turn, this emotional fatigue undermines language processing, recall, and overall performance [11, 42]. In turn, this emotional fatigue undermines language processing, recall, and overall performance. Our findings thus validate the central role of emotional exhaustion as a mediating mechanism: although teacher talk directly improves learning, it is the subsequent emotional exhaustion that mediates this impact [48, 82].

Moreover, our study shows that student self-efficacy, a key self-regulated aptitude which measures the level of confidence a student has in his or her capacity to shouldering responsibility and to perform in learning tasks in foreign languages can significantly mediate the effects of enjoyment and emotional exhaustion on language learning [45]. The research study established that high self-efficacy enhanced the impact of enjoyment among the students in the learning of English language compared to the low level of self-efficacy. When students have high self-efficacy they will therefore expend more effort and stay on task longer when faced with difficulties, thus improving their language learning [43, 56]. This finding resonates with Schunk and Pajares [48], who proved that self-efficacious students are also more manageable because they can recover and build on effective learning experiences turning enjoyment to enhanced learning. On the other hand, self-efficacy also plays the role of a moderator which helps to avoid the negative impact of the phenomenon of emotional exhaustion of foreign language learning outcomes. Stressed and fatigued students with high self-efficacy can easily overcome these aspects hence veering the negative impact of emotional exhaustion on cognitive involvement and achievement [43, 45, 56]. Our moderation analysis indicated that treatment effect of emotional exhaustion on English language attainment was significantly less negative among high self-efficacy students compared to those with low self-efficacy, consistent with the moderating effects of self-efficacy posited by Schwarzer and Hallum [83]. This implies that the improvement of self-efficacy could act as a buffer and can help students get back on track in terms of performance, even when they face pressure that comes with emotional stress.

Implications for practice

From this study, we can derive the following conclusions that can be insightful to educators, policymakers and researchers of language education. Several of these may be highlighted: First is the strong focus on teacher talk as a form of dialogic control that influences the emotional and cognitive participation of students. The more teachers create an interactive classroom environment (e.g., asking open-ended questions, engaging in discussions, using group activities), the more positively students may perceive teacher talk. This approach not only increases students’ enjoyment but also encourages active learning. Specifically, teacher talk that encourages active participation, questioning, and discussion can make students feel more involved in their learning. When students are given opportunities to respond, ask questions, and share opinions, they tend to enjoy the class more. Teachers can ask open-ended questions and facilitate peer interactions to enhance engagement. This study’s implications propose that through professional, enthusiastic, and warm-voiced teacher communications, not only is students’ enjoyment but also their lack of emotional stock diminishes, thus providing the better learning. This highlights the importance for of the professional development programs that helps teachers in proper communication strategies. In essence, with effective strategies and by creating a good environment that teachers cultivate in their classes, the motivation and ability of students to learn is heightened, thus leading to improved language mastery. Further, the moderating influence of student self-efficacy underlines the need to encourage the students, especially through enhancing their self-efficacy in learning the language. Teachers should work with students to identify their individual strengths and encourage them to leverage these strengths in language learning. Teacher can utilize language learning apps, interactive software, and online platforms that offer instant feedback can help students practice and see their progress in real-time. These tools can be motivating and help students develop confidence in their ability to learn at their own pace, fostering a sense of achievement as they use these platforms to improve their language skills. In this case, educators should avoid statements that compromise self-efficacy and instead, practice effective ways of rebuilding it; for instance giving negative feedback, setting realistic goal, and acknowledging small achievements. All the above strategies can help in relieving stress and in making the students strong enough to harness the positive emotions in enhancing of learning.

Limitations and direction for future research

There are several limitations that should be taken into consideration in our study. A key limitation of this study is the reliance on self-reported measures, which are subject to biases such as social desirability or self-perceived ability bias. Since participants were asked to report on their perceptions of teacher talk, enjoyment, emotional exhaustion, and self-efficacy, their responses may reflect what they believe is socially acceptable or desirable, rather than their actual experiences or feelings. Additionally, students may overestimate their own self-efficacy or downplay negative emotions like exhaustion to present themselves in a more favorable light. This type of bias can undermine the accuracy and validity of the findings, as the data may not fully capture the true emotional or cognitive states of the participants. To mitigate this, future studies could employ more objective measures, such as direct classroom observations or physiological indicators of emotional responses, such as classroom environment or teacher characteristics, on student perceptions and learning outcomes should be addressed, which would provide a more accurate reflection of the students’ emotional and cognitive states during learning.

Furthermore, the cross-sectional design of this study presents a significant limitation in terms of establishing causal relationships. Because data were collected at a single point in time, we cannot definitively conclude that perceptions of teacher talk lead to changes in student enjoyment or emotional exhaustion, nor can we establish how these factors influence language attainment over time. Longitudinal research, which tracks participants over a longer period, would be valuable for determining the directionality and temporal relationships between teacher talk, emotions, and language learning outcomes. For instance, researchers could measure these variables at multiple time points, helping to identify trends and causality, as well as how changes in emotional states or perceptions of teacher talk might influence learning attainment over time.

Furthermore, future studies could benefit from employing a variety of data collection methods, such as direct classroom observations or in-depth interviews, to triangulate the findings. By integrating these additional approaches, researchers can gather richer, more nuanced insights into student perceptions, allowing for a more comprehensive understanding of how teacher talk, emotional responses, and learning outcomes are interconnected. Observational studies could provide a first-hand account of teacher-student interactions, while interviews would offer students an opportunity to express their thoughts and feelings in their own words. This mixed-methods approach would help cross-verify data and reduce potential biases inherent in relying solely on self-reports, leading to more robust and reliable conclusions.

Another significant limitation of this study is its contextual and cultural boundaries. The sample was drawn from a specific university setting, and the findings may not be generalizable to other educational contexts, such as K-12 schools, or to different cultural environments. Students in different regions or educational systems may interpret teacher talk differently, and their emotional responses to the teaching environment could vary widely based on cultural norms and educational practices. For example, cultural attitudes toward authority, communication styles, and student-teacher interactions can all shape how students perceive and respond to teacher talk. Future studies should aim to include more diverse samples across different educational settings and cultural contexts to assess whether the observed patterns hold universally or if they are specific to particular environments. Conducting cross-cultural studies could help determine how teacher talk and students’ emotional responses interact in different cultural contexts, offering a broader understanding of these relationships. The study results could not be applied to all educational environments especially those located in countries where cultural perception regarding interactions between teachers and students and learning of languages are dissimilar. Another direction for future research is to test the results obtained in similar settings to verify the generalizability of the obtained results.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Graddol D. English next. Volume 62. British council London; 2006.

  2. Mahmoodi MH, Hosseiniyar S, Samoudi N. EFL teachers’ classroom management orientation, self-efficacy, burnout, and students’ L2 achievement. Profile Issues TeachersProfessional Dev. 2022;24(1):29–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Voller P. Does the teacher have a role in autonomous language learning? in Autonomy and independence in language learning. 2014, Routledge. pp. 98–113.

  4. Mackey A, Gass SM. Second Language research: methodology and design. Routledge; 2015.

  5. Chen G, et al. The link between student-perceived teacher talk and student enjoyment, anxiety and discursive engagement in the classroom. Br Edu Res J. 2020;46(3):631–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Khan AN, et al. Psychological dynamics of overqualification: career anxiety and decision commitment in STEM. BMC Psychol. 2024;12(1):1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Csikszentmihalyi M, Csikzentmihaly M. Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. Vol. 1990. 1990: Harper & Row New York.

  8. Pekrun R, et al. Measuring emotions in students’ learning and performance: the achievement emotions questionnaire (AEQ). Contemp Educ Psychol. 2011;36(1):36–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Muti MS. Exploring teacher talk in the classroom: A discourse analysis of instructional conversations. in International Conference English Language Education For Undergraduate Students. 2024.

  10. Sharpe T. How can teacher talk support learning? Linguistics Educ. 2008;19(2):132–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Maslach C, Schaufeli WB, Leiter MP. Job burnout. Ann Rev Psychol. 2001;52(1):397–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Brady KJ, et al. Describing the emotional exhaustion, depersonalization, and low personal accomplishment symptoms associated with Maslach burnout inventory subscale scores in US physicians: an item response theory analysis. J patient-reported Outcomes. 2020;4:1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Cao XF, et al. Leveraging social media capabilities and digital fluency to foster employee green innovation behavior: from a social cognitive theory perspective. Curr Psychol. 2024;43(38):29917–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Pekrun R, et al. Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learning and achievement: A program of qualitative and quantitative research. Educational Psychol. 2002;37(2):91–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Skaalvik EM, et al. Students’ perceptions of mathematics classroom goal structures: implications for perceived task values and study behavior. Soc Psychol Educ. 2017;20:543–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Khan NA, Zhang J, Akhtar M. Gig work Self-Efficacy and its effects on flexibility, career adoptability, and adoption in the metaverse: A moderated mediation model. Int J Human–Computer Interact, 2024: pp. 1–12.

  17. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. 1977;84(2):191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Ali A, Wang H, Johnson RE. Empirical analysis of shared leadership promotion and team creativity: an adaptive leadership perspective. J Organizational Behav. 2020;41(5):405–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Pajares F. Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Rev Educ Res. 1996;66(4):543–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Ellis NC. The dynamics of second Language emergence: cycles of Language use, Language change, and Language acquisition. Mod Lang J. 2008;92(2):232–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Fathi J et al. Exploring factors affecting foreign Language achievement: the role of growth mindset, self-efficacy, and L2 grit. Lang Teach Res, 2024:13621688241227603.

  22. Krashen S. The pleasure hypothesis. Georgetown University round table on languages and linguistics, 1994:299–302.

  23. Lightbown PM, Spada N. Teaching and learning L2 in the classroom: it’s about time. Lang Teach. 2020;53(4):422–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Swain M, Lapkin S. Problems in output and the cognitive processes they generate: A step towards second Language learning. Appl Linguist. 1995;16(3):371–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Mercer S, Dörnyei Z. Engaging Language Learners in Contemporary Classrooms. 2020.

  26. Deci EL, Ryan RM. Self-determination theory: A macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Can Psychol. 2008;49(3):182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Birdsong D. Ultimate attainment in second language acquisition. Language, 1992: pp. 706–755.

  28. Walsh S. Exploring classroom discourse: Language in action. 2011: Routledge.

  29. Thornbury S. About Language: tasks for teachers of english. Cambridge University Press; 1997.

  30. Kim SHO, Elder C. Target Language use in foreign Language classrooms: practices and perceptions of two native speaker teachers in new Zealand. Lang Cult Curriculum. 2008;21(2):167–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Cullen R. Teacher talk and the classroom context. ELT J. 1998;52(3):179–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Zhao X, Wang D. The role of enjoyment and boredom in shaping english Language achievement among ethnic minority learners. J Multiling Multicultural Dev, 2023: pp. 1–13.

  33. Long M. The role of the linguistic environment in second Language acquisition. Handbook of second language acquisition; 1996.

  34. Fredricks JA, Blumenfeld PC, Paris AH. School engagement: potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Rev Educ Res. 2004;74(1):59–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Lucardie D. The impact of fun and enjoyment on adult’s learning. Procedia-Social Behav Sci. 2014;142:439–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Dewaele J-M, MacIntyre PD. Foreign Language enjoyment and foreign Language classroom anxiety: the right and left feet of the Language learner. Posit Psychol SLA. 2016;215(236):9781783095360–010.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Li C, Zhang LJ, Jiang G. Conceptualisation and measurement of foreign Language learning burnout among Chinese EFL students. J Multiling Multicultural Dev. 2024;45(4):906–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Ali A, et al. Conservation of resources theory perspective of social media ostracism influence on lurking intentions. Behav Inform Technol. 2023;43(1):212–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Cao X et al. A social comparison perspective to investigating the influence of social media on employee creativity. Int J Human–Computer Interact, 2024: pp. 1–14.

  40. Cao X, et al. An investigation of the social media overload and academic performance. Educ Inform Technol. 2023;29(8):10303–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Wang Y, Wang N, Shen B. A network approach to Language learning burnout, negative emotions, and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching; 2024.

  42. Zhang Q, Sapp DA. The effect of perceived teacher burnout on credibility. Communication Res Rep. 2009;26(1):87–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Mills N. Self-efficacy in second language acquisition. Multiple perspectives on the self in SLA, 2014. 1: pp. 6–22.

  44. Huang SC, Chang SF. Self-Efficacy of English as a Second Language Learner: An Example of Four Learners. 1996.

  45. Raoofi S, Tan BH, Chan SH. Self-efficacy in second/foreign Language learning contexts. Engl Lang Teach. 2012;5(11):60–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Van Dinther M, Dochy F, Segers M. Factors affecting students’ self-efficacy in higher education. Educational Res Rev. 2011;6(2):95–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Graham CR. Blended learning systems. Handb Blended Learning: Global Perspect Local Designs. 2006;1:3–21.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Schunk DH, Pajares F. Self-Effi Cacy Th eory, in Handbook of motivation at school. Routledge; 2009. pp. 49–68.

  49. Hassankhani H, et al. The relationship between learning motivation and self efficacy among nursing students. Res Dev Med Educ. 2015;4(1):97–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Dicke T, et al. Self-efficacy in classroom management, classroom disturbances, and emotional exhaustion: A moderated mediation analysis of teacher candidates. J Educ Psychol. 2014;106(2):569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Schunk DH. Self-efficacy and achievement behaviors. Educational Psychol Rev. 1989;1:173–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Schunk DH. Self-Efficacy for Learning and Performance. 1996.

  53. Tschannen-Moran M, Hoy AW. Teacher efficacy: capturing an elusive construct. Teach Teacher Educ. 2001;17(7):783–805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Goddard RD, Tschannen-Moran M, Hoy WK. A multilevel examination of the distribution and effects of teacher trust in students and parents in urban elementary schools. Elementary School J. 2001;102(1):3–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Kim Y, Baylor AL. A social-cognitive framework for pedagogical agents as learning companions. Education Tech Research Dev. 2006;54:569–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Zimmerman BJ. Self-efficacy: an essential motive to learn. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2000;25(1):82–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: classic definitions and new directions. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2000;25(1):54–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Hair JF, et al. When to use and how to report the results of PLS-SEM. Eur Bus Rev. 2019;31(1):2–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Rafdinal W, Senalasari W. Predicting the adoption of mobile payment applications during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Bank Mark. 2021;39(6):984–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. West R et al. The state of English in higher education in Turkey. Ankara: British Council & TEPAV. 2015.

  61. Anderson NC, Chapin S, O’Connor C. Classroom discussions in math: A facilitator’s guide to support professional learning of discourse and the Common Core. Math Solutions; 2011.

  62. Michaels S, O’Connor C. Conceptualizing talk moves as tools: Professional development approaches for academically productive discussion. Socializing intelligence through talk and dialogue, 2015: pp. 347–362.

  63. Resnick LB, Schantz F. Re-thinking intelligence: schools that build the Mind. Eur J Educ. 2015;50(3):340–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Schaufeli WB, Bakker AB. Job demands, job resources, and their relationship with burnout and engagement: A multi-sample study. J Organizational Behavior: Int J Industrial Occup Organizational Psychol Behav. 2004;25(3):293–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Kutuk G, et al. The development and preliminary validation of a new measure of self-efficacy: questionnaire of self-efficacy in learning a foreign Language. ITL-International J Appl Linguistics. 2023;174(2):230–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Division C. o.E.C.f.C.C.-o.E.C.M.L., Common European framework of reference for Languages: learning, teaching, assessment. Cambridge University Press; 2001.

  67. Kumar D, et al. Psychological capital and innovative work behaviour: the role of mastery orientation and creative self-efficacy. Int J Hospitality Manage. 2022;102:103157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Prügl R, Spitzley DI. Responding to digital transformation by external corporate venturing: an enterprising family identity and communication patterns perspective. J Manage Stud. 2021;58(1):135–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Fornell C, Larcker DF. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J Mark Res. 1981;18:39–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Pavlou PA, El OA, Sawy. From IT leveraging competence to competitive advantage in turbulent environments: the case of new product development. Inform Syst Res. 2006;17(3):198–227.

  72. Rönkkö M, Cho E. An updated guideline for assessing discriminant validity. Organizational Res Methods. 2022;25(1):6–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Zhou Z, Jin X-L, Fang Y. Moderating role of gender in the relationships between perceived benefits and satisfaction in social virtual world continuance. Decis Support Syst. 2014;65:69–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Podsakoff NP. Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Ann Rev Psychol. 2012;63:539–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Hulland J, Baumgartner H, Smith KM. Marketing survey research best practices: evidence and recommendations from a review of JAMS articles. J Acad Mark Sci. 2018;46:92–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Cai Z, et al. Improving the agility of employees through enterprise social media: the mediating role of psychological conditions. Int J Inf Manag. 2018;38(1):52–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Yadama GN, Pandey S. Effect of sample size on goodness-fit of-fit indices in structural equation models. J Social Service Res. 1995;20(3–4):49–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Williams J. Confidence limits for the indirect effect: distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivar Behav Res. 2004;39(1):99–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Aiken LS, West SG, Reno RR. Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions. Sage; 1991.

  80. Tugade MM, Fredrickson BL. Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2004;86(2):320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Hobfoll SE. Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am Psychol. 1989;44(3):513–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Bandura A, Wessels S. Self-efficacy. 1994.

  83. Schwarzer R, Hallum S. Perceived teacher self-efficacy as a predictor of job stress and burnout: mediation analyses. Appl Psychol. 2008;57:152–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research work is not supported by any research funding.

Funding

This project is funded by Shanxi Province’s 2023 Higher Education Teaching Reform and Innovation Project “Research on the Innovation of English Teaching in Local Application-oriented University Based on Content and Language Integration(CLI) Fund No.: J20231877”.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Author “Peihong Wu” conducted the study, wrote the main manuscript text and prepared figures and tables. Author “Zhimin Wang” collected the data, and overall improve the quality of manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peihong Wu.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Conflict of interest

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted in accordance with the ethical principles stated in the Declaration of Helsinki. All study protocols were approved by ethics committee of School of Foreign Languages, Taiyuan University of Science and Technology. An informed consent was obtained from the adolescents and from their parent(s) or legal guardian(s).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

We declared there is no competing interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wu, P., Wang, Z. The impact of the student perception of teacher talk on student language learning attainment. BMC Psychol 13, 221 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-025-02508-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-025-02508-3

Keywords