Your privacy, your choice

We use essential cookies to make sure the site can function. We also use optional cookies for advertising, personalisation of content, usage analysis, and social media.

By accepting optional cookies, you consent to the processing of your personal data - including transfers to third parties. Some third parties are outside of the European Economic Area, with varying standards of data protection.

See our privacy policy for more information on the use of your personal data.

for further information and to change your choices.

Skip to main content

Measurement issues in longitudinal studies of mental health problems in children with neurodevelopmental disorders

Abstract

Purpose

To develop and test an approach for assessing the risk of bias in four measurement-related domains key to the study of mental health problem trajectories in children with neurodevelopmental disorders (NDD): (1) conceptual overlap between mental health problems and NDD diagnostic criteria, (2) over-reliance on a single informant, (3) unwarranted omission of the child’s perspective, and (4) the use of instruments not designed for or adapted to the population.

Methods

Building upon a previous systematic review, this study established supplementary criteria for assessing the risk of bias domains. Following this, the criteria were applied to measures used in 49 longitudinal studies of mental health problems in children with NDD.

Results

The general risk of bias across domains was rated as high in 57.1% of the 49 included studies. The highest risk of bias was seen in domain four (rated as high in 87.8% of studies) and the lowest in domain three (24.5%).

Conclusions

The risk of bias items enhance our understanding of the quality of the evidence about mental health problem trajectories in children with NDD. The methodological quality of future research can be increased by selecting conceptually clear scales developed for the population - preferably in the form of cognitively accessible self-report scales - and adopting a multi-informant approach.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Neurodevelopmental disorders (NDD) such as intellectual disability (ID), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and cerebral palsy (CP) have repeatedly been linked to heightened levels of mental health problems and mental disorders across childhood [1,2,3,4,5,6]. However, the measurement of longitudinal trajectories of mental health problems in children with NDD is associated with specific methodological challenges, relating to the interplay among the longitudinal design, study group characteristics, and the standard methods for measuring mental health problems in children. In a recent systematic review of longitudinal mental health problem trajectories in children with NDD [7], we observed that the instruments and checklists used to assess the risk of bias [8] were inadequate in addressing some of these challenges. Specifically, four domains related to aspects of outcome measurement stood out: (1) conceptual overlap between mental health problems and NDD diagnostic criteria, (2) over-reliance on a single informant, (3) unwarranted omission of the child’s perspective, and (4) the use of instruments not designed for or adapted to the population. The assessment of these domains, in our review, required more detailed data to be extracted from the included studies, and additional consideration of risks of bias. The present study aimed to develop an approach for evaluating the risk of bias in the four domains and to investigate the extent to which the four domains influence the validity of the findings about longitudinal trajectories of mental health problems in children with NDD.

The first of these specific risks concerns the potential conceptual overlap between mental health problems and NDD constructs [9]. It stems from the fact that mental illness, mental health problems, and mental disorders may overlap, depending on how they are defined [9]. In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM-5 [10]), a mental disorder is defined as a syndrome “characterized by clinically significant disturbance in an individual’s cognition, emotion regulation, or behavior that reflects a dysfunction in the psychological, biological, or development processes underlying mental functioning” (p. 20). According to this definition, mental disorders encompass conditions that are typically regarded as mental illnesses (i.e., anxiety disorders and major depressive disorder), NDD (i.e., disorders typically manifested early in development, characterised by developmental deficits that produce impairments of personal, social, academic, or occupational functioning [10]), and other diagnoses. Mental health problems are commonly regarded as conceptually similar to mental illness, but as a broader construct, also covering milder problems and distress not meeting the criteria for a mental disorder [9]. In the present review, both internalising – (e.g., depression and anxiety) and externalising problems (e.g., aggressive behaviour) were considered as part of the mental health problem umbrella. Although this meant further widening of the mental health problems concept, it was necessary to enable comparisons of different aspects of emotional and behavioural problems. We also adopted a broad understanding of NDD by including childhood sensory and motor disorders, brain injuries acquired in childhood, and other diagnoses associated with sensory, motor, and mental impairments, in addition to the diagnoses listed as NDD in the DSM-5 [10] or the ICD-11 [11]. Many diagnoses involving such impairments that are not listed as NDD in the DSM-5 [10] arguably share important characteristics with those that are. For example, CP [12] and childhood hearing loss [13] are also characterised by developmental deficits that may produce impairments in different aspects of functioning. Moreover, conditions such as spina bifida [14] and CP [15] have previously been described as NDDs, with the latter showing genetic overlap with other NDDs, including intellectual disability and autism.

Importantly, an overlap between the mental health problem studied and the NDD is not problematic per se, but it may be, depending on the study’s aim and the interpretation of results. There is substantial symptomatic overlap across the mental disorders listed in the DSM-5 [16]. For example, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and major depressive disorder both involve symptoms regarding concentration difficulties [10], and any instrument measuring the full symptomatology of any of these constructs will inevitably tap into the other construct. Further, overlap may occur when an instrument is applied to measure a mental health construct containing items that are identically worded, or similar to, criteria used to diagnose the population studied, and when results – despite this overlap – are reported as a separate mental health construct. For example, the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; [17]), assesses emotional and behavioural problems in four specific scales (emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity, and peer relationship problems). Notably, the SDQ can be used to screen for specific NDD, such as ASD and ADHD (e.g., [18, 19]), and to measure emotional and behavioural problems more broadly in children with different NDD (e.g., [20,21,22]). The specific scales can be combined to form the broad-band scales of internalising (emotional symptoms and peer relationships problems) and externalising problems (conduct problems and hyperactivity) and a total difficulties scale (the sum of all four). Each specific scale consists of five items, and in the case of the peer relationship problems scale, several items are either closely related to or overlapping diagnostic criteria for ASD (e.g., “Rather solitary, tends to play alone”). Whether this overlap is problematic or not is related to which scales are reported and how the results are interpreted. For example, when the score of the peer relationships problems is reported as an indication of ASD the overlap is necessary (e.g., [18]) and when subscales with overlap are purposely omitted (e.g., [23, 24]) the risk for bias due to overlap is avoided. However, when the broad-band scales or the total scale are reported as an indication of a mental health problem construct there is a risk that disability-related difficulties are confused with mental health problems. This could lead to inflated scores which could contribute to incorrect conclusions about differences between groups.

The second domain covered in the review concerns bias arising from an over-reliance on a single type of informant in reporting the mental health problems in focus. Evidence shows that correlations between different types of informants (e.g., parents, teachers, children) can be low to modest depending on the combination [25,26,27]. Low inter-informant correlation does not necessarily mean that one informant is right and the other one is wrong or implies measurement error [28]. More likely, it relates to systematic differences among informants, such as the contexts in which they observe behaviour [29]. For example, a child may display hyperactivity in school but not at home. Relying on a single informant might provide an incomplete picture of mental health problems. A multi-informant approach is often advised to reduce bias when studying child mental health issues [26, 30]. However, the number and types of informants needed to provide a valid representation may depend on factors like the child’s age, as suggested by the larger discrepancy between informants seen in older as compared to younger children [25].

The third risk of bias domain, which can be considered a special case of the second domain, involves the exclusion of the child’s perspective, and the use of a parent as a sole informant. Exclusion of the child’s perspective might lead to bias through the depression-distortion hypothesis, i.e., the tendency for depressed mothers to exaggerate descriptions of child problems [31]. This risk is highlighted separately in this review because some aspects of mental health problems are inherently subjective. This subjectivity could be an explanation for the larger informant discrepancies observed in internalising problems, such as anxiety and depression, as compared to externalising problems [25,26,27]. Furthermore, there is a strong ethical argument for including the child’s perspective whenever possible. According to the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child [32], every child has a right to be heard in matters that concern the child. However, it is not realistic to expect all children to be able to self-report mental health problems. For children with NDD, the disability itself may be associated with problems with self-reporting, for example, difficulties with interpreting questions, retrieving information, and generating responses, related to underlying cognitive processes such as long-term memory, working memory, and judgment [33, 34]. Some children with NDD, such as those with severe-profound ID, by definition, have a level of impairment in cognitive and communicative functions [10], which makes the use of self-rating questionnaires improbable [35]. Similarly, young children may not have developed the necessary level of cognitive functioning to self-rate on mental health problems, regardless of NDD or not (see for example [36, 37]). However, determining the specific age and cognitive level at which children’s self-report reaches acceptable validity is challenging. For example, Varni, Limbers, and Burwinkle [38] demonstrated that typically developing children as young as five years may be able to make valid reports on their health-related quality of life using the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory™ (PedsQL™). However, a later analysis of PedsQL™ data showed insufficient psychometric properties for many children between the ages of five and seven years [39]. For children with mild-moderate ID, some evidence indicates that self-rating of mental health problems may be feasible from 11 years, using standard self-rating instruments such as the SDQ [40] or the Youth Self-Report [41], with some adaptations made to the procedure (i.e., questions administered as an interview [40, 41], allowing item content to be explained [41]). Any effort to identify a specific and universal age and cognitive functioning threshold for child self-ratings is likely to fail since validity is also influenced by material and procedural factors [42]. Still, if a child’s self-report is not sought, when possible, an important perspective on the mental health problem is missing. Importantly, this is not the same as saying that there is no merit to parent reports, but rather that parent and child reports are not interchangeable.

The fourth and final risk of bias domain concerns the appropriateness of instruments used to measure mental health problems in the population. Many scales, such as the SDQ and the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; [43, 44]), were originally developed for typically developing children. Using these scales could be problematic if manifestations of mental health problems differ between typically developing children and those with NDD. For example, it has been argued that the number and type of symptoms for some psychiatric disorders need to be adapted for use with people with ID [45]. This would imply that questions in diagnostic interviews and screening questionnaires need to be phrased differently. Additionally, all questionnaires and interview procedures, presume some level of cognitive and communicative functioning in respondents. A cognitively accessible design reduces cognitive demands and supports cognitive processes to enable respondents to interpret and respond to assessment items as intended [46]. For example, in many scales, the respondent is to consider a time frame of several weeks or months when rating items. A valid response to the items in such scales presumes a comparatively high level of episodic memory functioning in the respondent, which should pose a bigger challenge to children with impairments in memory functions, such as those with ID [47], than children without memory impairments. Self-assessment could be made a feasible option for a larger proportion of children with NDD through the development of more accessible instruments. This could be achieved by adapting well-established scales to the needs of children with NDD (e.g., [48]) or developing new scales suitable for the target group (e.g., [49]). Of course, not all scales need to be changed to be valid for use in specific groups of children with NDD, as demonstrated by the tentative evidence concerning the use of SDQ and YSL in children with mild-moderate ID [40, 41]. But in samples where cognitive impairments vary or are unknown, higher cognitive accessibility should increase the likelihood of valid responses. This is also true for parent-rated versions of scales. In ID [50], for example, there is a strong genetic component, indicating that cognitive support needs are likely to be expected in many parents of children with ID as well as the children themselves.

In summary, the four domains (see Table 1) involve known methodological challenges that researchers and clinicians attempting to track the longitudinal change in mental health problems in children with NDD need to consider and manage. Common risk of bias appraisal tools like the checklists used in the Critical Appraisal Skills Programme [8] do not provide detailed instructions for assessing bias in these specific yet important domains. Given that many other design features need to be considered when assessing the risk of bias, there is a possibility that these NDD-specific questions may be overlooked. Hence, the purpose of this study was (1) to develop an approach to assessing the risk of bias associated with the four identified domains and (2) to assess the risk of these biases in a recently conducted systematic review of longitudinal trajectories of mental health problems in children with NDD [7]. The research questions for this study were:

  1. 1

    Is the risk for overlap between the outcome measures and the criteria used to define the study group (i.e., the NDD) dealt with satisfactorily?

  2. 2

    To what extent has a multi-informant approach been taken to capture variation in mental health problem expression in different contexts?

  3. 3

    (a) Is the child’s perspective represented in the assessment of the mental health problem? (b) When not, is there a reasonable basis for excluding child self-assessment?

  4. 4

    Are the instruments and procedures designed to be cognitively accessible, or have they been adapted in some way to the specific needs of the study group?

Table 1 Four domains presenting challenges in measuring longitudinal changes in mental health problems in children with neurodevelopmental disorders (NDD)

The present study builds on findings from an earlier systematic review of longitudinal mental health problem trajectories in children with NDD [7]. That review identified indications of methodological issues that were not adequately captured by the standard risk of bias tool employed. To better understand the scope and nature of these issues, additional data extraction from the included studies was required. By addressing these four key questions, this study sought to contribute to a better understanding of the methodological weaknesses and strengths of the field of mental health problem trajectories in children with NDD. The findings will be summarised and used to critically evaluate how the field has dealt with the challenges posed.

The study protocol for the systematic review of mental health problem trajectories in children with NDD [7] was registered in PROSPERO (142,412). Some aspects of the design, e.g., search strategy and eligibility criteria, are summarised or appended as supplementary material in the current review, but a more comprehensive description can be found in Danielsson et al. [7]. Taken together, the reviews adhere to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines [51].

Search strategy

Searches were performed in PsycINFO, ERIC, Web of Science, PubMed, and CINAHL in September 2019 and June 2021 with combinations of words (i.e., synonyms, examples, or MeSH-terms) representing the constructs “mental health”, “disability”, “longitudinal”, and “child”. The searches resulted in 94,662 records, which were reduced to 72,582 with duplicates removed. Another 8,599 were identified by going through the reference lists of relevant reviews. The records were then screened in a three-stage process based on title, abstract, and full-length texts. Due to the large number of identified records, 22 reviewers were involved in the process. An overview of the flow of records through the study is provided in Supplementary Fig. 1, and the detailed eligibility criteria are outlined in Supplementary Table 1.

Selection criteria

Studies were included based on the eligibility criteria found in Supplementary Table 1. In short, longitudinal studies of mental health problems (defined broadly) in children under 19 years of age with NDD were included. Studies with at least three waves of data collection (with two or more years between the first and last wave) were included. Studies, where the mental health problems of interest were the explicit target of an intervention, were excluded, along with papers written in any language other than English. No limitations regarding publication year were applied.

Data extraction process

For the original review [7], the extraction of relevant information was done independently by random pairs of reviewers (n = 22) and synthesised by a third reviewer. In cases where the original reviewers disagreed, the third did an independent extraction of data and made a final decision based on all three sources. For the current study, key variables relating to measurement constructs/concepts, respondents, and cognitive accessibility were added, and the additional data for these variables were extracted by one investigator (M.I.). In ambiguous cases, another investigator (H.D. or C.I.) was consulted and a consensus decision was made. Information about diagnostic criteria and instruments was retrieved from relevant diagnostic manuals like the DSM-5 and instrument manuals. Most of the data reported were extracted from the included studies for the current study. However, some of the study characteristics (e.g., study populations’ mean age at the first data point), are included in the present study for context, even though they have been reported previously [7].

Risk of bias assessment protocol

The general risk of bias assessment of the included studies has been previously reported by Danielsson et al. [7]. For the current study, the first author (M.I.), developed a supplementary tool to assess the risk of bias in four domains addressing specific challenges related to the measurement of longitudinal trajectories of mental health problems in children with NDD. Specific assessment criteria (questions) were formulated for each domain to guide the evaluation of the risk of bias. The risk of bias for each domain was rated on a three-point scale based on the responses to the underlying questions (Table 2 provides a detailed description of the assessment for each domain): “Low” (indicating minimal risk), “High” (indicating elevated risk), and “Unclear” (reflecting uncertainties in the bias assessment, either due to mixed findings or insufficient information). The overall risk of bias, across the four domains, was also assessed for each study according to the following principles: (1) if any level of risk of bias (“low”, “unclear”, or “high”) was assigned more frequently than any of the others in the four domains then the overall risk of bias was rated at that level, (2) if “high” and “low” was assigned two times each the overall risk of bias was set to “unclear”, and (3) if two domains were considered to have a “high” or “low” risk of bias and the other two an “unclear”, the overall risk of bias was described as “unclear”. It is important to underscore that this supplement is not intended as a standalone instrument to assess the total risk of bias for a study, but as a complement to standard instruments, for example, the Cochrane Collaboration’s tool [52] or the Critical Appraisal Skills Programme checklist for cohort studies [8].

Table 2 A supplementary risk of bias tool for studies assessing mental health problems in children with NDD

Data analysis

Analyses were carried out in R [53] with RStudio [54]. The R packages papaja [55] and robvis [56] were used to compile the manuscript and to make the risk of bias figures. The data extracted from the included studies and a reproducible version of the manuscript is available at https://osf.io/hjrqc/.

Results

A total of 49 original studies were identified through the screening and eligibility process and included in the current review (see Table 3 for an overview of the included studies). Of these, at least 18 reported data from participants that, based on the name of the project or resemblances in participant characteristics, were likely to also have been participants in one or more of the other included studies. Disregarding this overlap, this review includes data from 9,446 participating children. The participants’ mean age ranged from 0.51 to 12.30 years at the first wave of data collection and 4.50 to 23.20 years at the last. The mean number of data collection waves was 5 (range 3 to 17) and the mean total length of follow-up was 5.62 years (range 2.00 to 16.74 years). A total of 148 scale scores were identified when counting the scales reported on the most general level in each included study, i.e., specific subscales were only counted in the absence of a reported total scale score or broad-band scale score. The scores were derived from 34 different instruments.

Table 3 Characteristics of the included studies

Risk of bias domain 1: Conceptual overlap

There was some level of overlap between one or more items and diagnostic criteria in 33.8% of the identified outcomes. Table 4 demonstrates examples of overlaps in two of the included studies and how the risk of bias was assessed. An overview of all studies with at least one overlap (44.9% of studies) is displayed in Supplementary Table 2. In studies where an overlap was identified, 77.3% contained at least one outcome with an overlap that was neither addressed nor discussed. The most frequently occurring instrument-diagnosis combination with a conceptual overlap was SDQ and ASD (n = 3). ADHD was the diagnosis where diagnostic criteria most commonly overlapped with at least one item in a reported mental health problem outcome (32.0% of the identified overlaps when including both pure ADHD groups and groups with ADHD and co-occurring diagnoses), followed by ASD (24.0%), and developmental disabilities (16.0%). ADHD, ASD, and developmental disabilities accounted for a larger proportion of the studies with overlap (72.0%) than would have been expected by the size of their combined share of the study groups in the included studies (30.4%).

Table 4 Examples of conceptual overlap between mental health problem outcomes and diagnostic criteria from two of the included studies

Risk of bias domain 2: Multi-informant approach

The risk of bias due to the lack of relevant perspectives on the mental health problems outcome was rated as high in 79.6% of the included studies (see Table 5 for examples of how the risk of bias was assessed in this domain and Supplementary Table 3 for an overview of all the included studies). Information about the mental health problem of interest was collected from multiple informants in 12.2% of the studies and relying on one informant was deemed justifiable, due to the young age of the population, in 8.2%.

Table 5 Informants recruited in four of the included studies and an assessment of the appropriateness of the informant recruitment approach based on the age of the participants

Risk of bias domain 3: Omission of the child’s perspective

The risk of bias due to a lack of the child’s perspective on the child’s mental health problems was rated as high in 24.5% of studies (see Table 6 for examples of how the risk of bias was assessed in this domain and Supplementary Table 4 for an overview of all the included studies). The child’s perspective was missing in 87.8% of the studies, and of these, child self-rating was deemed theoretically feasible in 53.5% based on a combination of participant age and reported level of intellectual functioning.

Table 6 Assessment of the feasibility of including the child’s perspective on the mental health problems outcomes in four of the included studies

Risk of bias domain 4: The use of instruments designed for or adapted to children with NDD

Only 8.8% of the different instruments applied were originally designed for use in children with NDD: the Aberrant Behavior Checklist, the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised, and the Scale for Emotional Development-Revised. No study reported that adaptations had been made to any instrument to make them more accessible or in other ways suitable for children with NDD (see Table 7 for examples of how the risk of bias was assessed in this domain and Supplementary Table 5 for an overview of all the included studies).

Table 7 Scales measuring mental health problems across four of the included studies and their suitability for use in the studied populations

Overall risk of bias in the four domains

Overall bias across domains was rated as high in 57.1%, unclear in 28.6%, and low in 14.3% of the 49 included studies (see Fig. 1). The domain with the highest proportion of studies rated as having a high risk was the fourth domain, i.e., bias due to the use of instruments not developed for or adapted to children with NDD (high risk of bias in 87.8% of the studies) and the third domain had the fewest risks, i.e., the unwarranted omission of the child’s perspective (high risk of bias in 24.5% of the included studies). All but one [96] of the included studies had a high risk of bias in one or more of the four domains (Supplementary Fig. 2 displays the risk of bias at the individual study level).

Fig. 1
figure 1

The overall risk of bias for each of the four domains: D1, overlap between mental health problem outcomes and characteristics of the study group; D2, insufficient informants; D3, unwarranted omission of the child’s perspective; and D4, use of instruments not designed for or adapted to the study group (red reflects “high” risk of bias, yellow “unclear”, green “low”, and blue “no information”)

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to develop and test an approach for assessing the risk of bias in four domains that are of particular importance in longitudinal studies of mental health problems in children with NDD and to assess how common these problems are in the field. Most notably, the results showed that some degree of bias related to these measurement domains was present in almost all of the included studies. Of these four domains, the most frequent concern was the use of instruments not designed for, or adapted to, children with NDD, followed by the restricted number of informants and perspectives represented in the mental health problem data. The risk of bias due to a lack of the child’s perspective and/or overlap between outcomes and the diagnostic criteria used to define the study group was lower compared to the other two domains but was still a substantial issue for the field as a whole.

The results show that conceptual overlap, i.e., mental health problems not being clearly distinguished from NDD diagnostic criteria, is a common concern in studies with populations with symptom-based diagnoses, such as ADHD and ASD, and to a lesser extent in those where diagnoses are based on etiology, such as pediatric traumatic brain injury or Fragile-X. In terms of outcomes, the broad-band internalising and externalising, or total scale scores of SDQ and the CBCL family of scales are often involved in cases with overlap. These scales were developed to screen broadly for problematic levels of emotional and behavioural difficulties in typically developing children and young people and, as such, do not differentiate between symptoms relating to NDD and other disorders or emotional problems on the broad-band levels. There are however subscales, such as the emotional problems scale of SDQ, where the risk for overlap is much lower than the internalising broad-band scale, which also encompasses the peer problems subscale with items very closely related to some NDDs.

The conceptual overlap is, when present, often not discussed and/or adjusted for in the analyses or through other design elements. When addressed, the approach to deal with it spans from mentioning the overlap as a limitation in the discussion [95] to clearly stating that scales with substantial overlap should be interpreted as NDD-related difficulties rather than additional mental health problems [63] and running analyses with and without items with an overlap to get a picture of their influence on the results [90]. The consequence of the conceptual overlap in the field is two-fold. Firstly, it could mean that the levels of mental health problems (as something separated from difficulties relating to NDD) are exaggerated in some groups of children with NDD. Secondly, it makes it hard to tell if a longitudinal change in the measured outcomes reflects changes in NDD-related difficulties or a separate mental health problem. For example, the natural course of NDD-specific difficulties, such as the tendency of a decreasing rate of hyperactivity over time in childhood ADHD [78, 106], risks distorting a mental health problems trajectory if the scale used includes items related to hyperactivity.

A clear majority of studies reported data from only one informant, most frequently a parent, despite recurrent recommendations to apply a multi-informant approach when assessing mental health in children [26, 30]. However, there were exceptions, such as Lahey et al. [78], in which three perspectives (child, teacher, and parent) on the mental health problems being investigated were combined. A restricted number of perspectives in a single study could be less of a problem if the field as a whole had a reasonable distribution of different perspectives. However, as seen in the results, there is an over-reliance on parents in the field today. Direct observations in the children’s natural contexts by researchers were not applied in any of the studies. This reliance on parent-reported data risks under-reporting of behaviours more typically displayed in other contexts than at home, such as problems between peers. Another risk is that parent ratings may be influenced by the parent’s mental health status [31]. This could be especially problematic for parents of children with NDD since they often report symptoms of depression, poor sleep quality, and stress [107]. A specific challenge in studies with a longitudinal design is that the most valid combination of methods and informants changes over time as the child develops. As argued by Rosema et al. [88], parents of younger children are likely to have more knowledge about their child’s mental health problems than parents of adolescents. One possible solution to this dilemma, demonstrated by Lahey et al. [78], is to add, rather than replace, informants as the child grows older.

The child’s perspective was missing in about a quarter of studies where child self-rating was deemed theoretically possible based on the participant’s age and level of intellectual functioning. Since some aspects of mental health problems are intrinsically covert (subjective), and difficult to measure without having the child describe their mental health (as pointed out by Woodruff-Borden et al. [103]), omitting the child’s perspective risks leading to a skewed picture with an overemphasis on overt behaviours. In the long run, this could lead to a self-fulfilling prophecy, where externalising problems are more often included as outcomes than internalising problems based on results from earlier studies. If the unwarranted omission of the child’s perspective is unevenly distributed between studies involving children with NDD and typical development, it follows that it could be difficult to disentangle real differences in profiles of emotional and behavioural differences between the groups from artifacts stemming from the methodological differences.

No examples of self-rating instruments specifically adapted to or designed to be cognitively accessible were identified in the current review. Very few of the scales were explicitly developed for use in the NDD population. Some instruments were claimed to have adequate psychometric properties when used in children with NDD but it was beyond the scope of the current review to go through all evidence on the psychometric properties of the included scales when used in the NDD population and evaluate the validity of such claims. Still, the use of instruments not developed for the population targeted in a study can lead to problems conceptually and practically. For example, mental health problems may have atypical presentations in children with NDD [45, 108], which means that questions may need to be phrased differently than with typically developing children. Further, when self-report is sought, scales need to be carefully designed to optimise cognitive accessibility.

Limitations

The validity and generalisability of the results of the present review are influenced by a few limitations that need to be discussed. First, even though the definitions and operationalisations applied were well-grounded, it should be noted that there may be reasons for considering other specific thresholds which would have led to slightly different outcomes. For example, a rather conservative definition of mental health problem-NDD overlap was applied, in that only explicit overlaps between diagnostic criteria and items in scales were considered. However, many etiology-based diagnoses are also closely linked to specific behavioural profiles, e.g., Fragile-X with ID and ASD [109]. Widening the definition of overlap to include difficulties typically associated with a disability would have resulted in more overlap being identified. At the same time, such a definition would have led to difficulties in drawing a clear line between NDD-related difficulties and common co-existing difficulties. Second, the present review did not quantify the extent of conceptual overlap in each of the included studies and therefore does not give a full picture of the risk of bias in that domain. Some of the studies used scales with hundreds of items whereof only a few overlapped diagnostic criteria, while other scales were much shorter and had more items with overlap. Future research will have to further investigate the exact extent of the problem. Third, the most frequently used risk of bias level across the four domains was used when calculating the overall risk of bias across domains, rather than generalising the highest risk of bias seen in a specific domain to the overall level. The reason for choosing this approach was that it allowed important variability to be exposed overall: generalising from the highest-rated item would have risked all included articles deemed to have the same (high) level of bias overall. Finally, the systematic search for evidence on which this study is based was conducted in 2021, hence more recent publications may have addressed these potential risks of bias more fully. However, the main aim of the current study was to develop and try out an approach for assessing these specific risks of bias rather than summarising the most recent evidence in the field of longitudinal mental health problem trajectories in children with NDD.

Clinical implications and future research

The overarching recommendation emanating from the results is that methods for collecting information on mental health problems in children with NDD could be chosen with more consideration than appears to have been done to date to avoid a partly skewed picture of mental health problems in both clinical and research settings. Several steps need to be taken to reduce the risk of bias in future studies. When selecting which scale(s) to use, it is important to:

  1. 1

    Choose conceptually clear scales and subscales.

  2. 2

    Prioritise self-report over parent-report, especially for internalising problems. When considering self-report, it is important to acknowledge that factors other than child-related factors determine whether it is feasible or not. The cognitive accessibility of scales is equally important and should be considered along with validity when deciding between scales.

  3. 3

    As with typically developing children, a multi-informant approach is recommended, especially but not only for older children who spend a large part of the day at school, with peers, or in other contexts without parents. In longitudinal studies where the first data collecting point takes place in early childhood, it is advised that self-report, teacher-report, and/or direct observations are added as the child grows older. Future studies need to investigate the barriers to applying a multi-informant approach in longitudinal studies of mental health problems in children with NDD.

  4. 4

    When no valid and accessible scale exists for a specific construct, a primary focus ought to be to develop one or adapt a scale originally developed for typically developing children to fit the needs of children with NDD. In research, more attention needs to be directed toward the challenge of developing and validating cognitively accessible self-report scales and procedures to assess mental health problems.

Finally, it is recommended that the four domains addressed in the current review should be considered whenever assessing the risk of bias in studies of mental health problems, and related constructs, in children with NDD in future systematic reviews. Without consideration of these additional potential risks of bias in this type of study, we may overestimate the quality of the evidence available.

Conclusions

The present study aimed to develop an approach for critically reviewing four measurement-related domains in studies investigating longitudinal trajectories of mental health problems in children with NDD, as well as to assess the risk of bias in these domains within the literature. All but one of the included studies had a high level of risk of bias in one or more domains, most commonly (1) the use of instruments not designed for or adapted to children with NDD, and in descending order, (2) an insufficient number of informants and perspectives represented in the mental health problem data, (3) overlap between the mental health problem outcomes and the diagnostic criteria used to define the study group, and (4) a lack of the child’s perspective. Taken together, these risks of bias could lead to a skewed picture of the mental health problems of children with NDD, through processes leading to both over- (e.g., conceptual overlap) and underestimation (e.g., use of instruments not developed for children with NDD). To minimise these problems in future research and clinical contexts, it is advised that instruments and procedures are chosen following a few guiding principles. Researchers and clinicians should seek to include multiple perspectives on the mental health issue of interest, use scales without conceptual overlap, preferably developed for children with NDD, and wherever possible in the form of cognitively accessible self-report scales. If no such scales exist, the development and validation of new scales should be a priority.

Data availability

The data that was extracted from the included studies and a reproducible version of the manuscript, including the code, are available at https://osf.io/hjrqc/.

References

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the systematic review.

  1. Glasson EJ, Buckley N, Chen W, Leonard H, Epstein A, Skoss R, Jacoby P, Blackmore AM, Bourke J, Downs J. Systematic Review and Meta-analysis: Mental Health in Children With Neurogenetic Disorders Associated With Intellectual Disability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;59:1036–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Hudson CC, Hall L, Harkness KL. Prevalence of Depressive Disorders in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Meta-Analysis. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2019;47:165–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. van Steensel FJA, Bögels SM, Perrin S. Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14:302.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Downs J, Blackmore AM, Epstein A, Skoss R, Langdon K, Jacoby P, Whitehouse AJO, Leonard H, Rowe PW, Glasson EJ. The prevalence of mental health disorders and symptoms in children and adolescents with cerebral palsy: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2018;60:30–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Buckley N, Glasson EJ, Chen W, et al. Prevalence estimates of mental health problems in children and adolescents with intellectual disability: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2020;54:970–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Meinzer MC, Pettit JW, Viswesvaran C. The co-occurrence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and unipolar depression in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34:595–607.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Danielsson H, Imms C, Ivarsson M, et al. A Systematic Review of Longitudinal Trajectories of Mental Health Problems in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disabilities. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-023-09914-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Critical Appraisal Skills Programme. CASP cohort study checklist. 2018.

  9. Granlund M, Imms C, King G, et al. Definitions and operationalization of mental health problems, wellbeing and participation constructs in children with ndd: Distinctions and clarifications. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18:1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th ed. 2013.

  11. World Health Organization. ICD-11 international classification of diseases 11th revision. 2022.

  12. Rosenbaum P, Paneth N, Leviton A, Goldstein M, Bax M, Damiano D, Dan B, Jacobsson B. A report: The definition and classification of cerebral palsy April 2006. Dev Med Child Neurol Suppl. 2007;49:8–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Lieu JEC, Kenna M, Anne S, Davidson L. Hearing Loss in Children: A Review. JAMA. 2020;324:2195–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Fletcher JM, Brei TJ. Introduction: Spina Bifida—A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2010;16:1–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. van Eyk CL, Fahey MC, Gecz J. Redefining cerebral palsies as a diverse group of neurodevelopmental disorders with genetic aetiology. Nat Rev Neurol. 2023;19:542–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Forbes MK, Neo B, Nezami OM, Fried EI, Faure K, Michelsen B, Twose M, Dras M. Elemental psychopathology: Distilling constituent symptoms and patterns of repetition in the diagnostic criteria of the DSM-5. 2023. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/u56p2.

  17. Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A Research Note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Vugteveen J, De Bildt A, Hartman CA, Timmerman ME. Using the Dutch multi-informant Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to predict adolescent psychiatric diagnoses. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018;27:1347–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:534–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Hastings SE, Hastings RP, Swales MA, Hughes JC. Emotional and behavioural problems of children with autism spectrum disorder attending mainstream schools. International Journal of Developmental Disabilities. 2022;68:633–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Murray CA, Hastings RP, Totsika V. Clinical utility of the parent-reported Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a screen for emotional and behavioural difficulties in children and adolescents with intellectual disability. Br J Psychiatry. 2021;218:323–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Simonoff E, Jones CRG, Baird G, Pickles A, Happé F, Charman T. The persistence and stability of psychiatric problems in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54:186–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Totsika V, Hastings RP, Emerson E, Lancaster GA, Berridge DM. A population-based investigation of behavioural and emotional problems and maternal mental health: Associations with autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:91–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Salomone E, Kutlu B, Derbyshire K, McCloy C, Hastings RP, Howlin P, Charman T. Emotional and behavioural problems in children and young people with autism spectrum disorder in specialist autism schools. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2014;8:661–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Achenbach TM, McConaughy SH, Howell CT. Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychol Bull. 1987;101:213–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. De Los RA, Augenstein TM, Wang M, Thomas SA, Drabick DAG, Burgers DE, Rabinowitz J. The validity of the multi-informant approach to assessing child and adolescent mental health. Psychol Bull. 2015;141:858–900.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Duhig AM, Renk K, Epstein MK, Phares V. Interparental Agreement on Internalizing, Externalizing, and Total Behavior Problems: A Meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2000;7:435–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Achenbach TM. Commentary: Definitely More Than Measurement Error: But How Should We Understand and Deal With Informant Discrepancies? J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40:80–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. De Los RA. Introduction to the Special Section: More Than Measurement Error: Discovering Meaning Behind Informant Discrepancies in Clinical Assessments of Children and Adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40:1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Kraemer HC, Measelle JR, Ablow JC, Essex MJ, Boyce WT, Kupfer DJ. A New Approach to Integrating Data From Multiple Informants in Psychiatric Assessment and Research: Mixing and Matching Contexts and Perspectives. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1566–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Chi TC, Hinshaw SP. Mother-child relationships of children with ADHD: The role of maternal depressive symptoms and depression-related distortions. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2002;30:387–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Convention on the Rights of the Child. Treaty No. 27531. New York: United Nations; 1989. Available from: https://www.ohchr.org/en/instruments-mechanisms/instruments/convention-rights-child. Accessed 11 Feb 2025.

  33. Beddow PA. Accessibility Theory for Enhancing the Validity of Test Results for Students with Special Needs. Int J Disabil Dev Educ. 2012;59:97–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Fujiura GT, the RRTC Expert Panel on Health Measurement. Self-Reported Health of People with Intellectual Disability. Intellect Dev Disabil. 2012;50:352–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Hartley SL, MacLean WE. A review of the reliability and validity of Likert-type scales for people with intellectual disability. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2006;50:813–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Ambridge B, Wearing H. The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Dev Psychol. 2004;40:177–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Ghetti S, Bunge SA. Neural changes underlying the development of episodic memory during middle childhood. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2012;2:381–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Varni JW, Limbers CA, Burwinkle TM. How young can children reliably and validly self-report their health-related quality of life?: An analysis of 8,591 children across age subgroups with the PedsQL 4.0 Generic Core Scales. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2007;5:1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Conijn JM, Smits N, Hartman EE. Determining at What Age Children Provide Sound Self-Reports: An Illustration of the Validity-Index Approach. Assessment. 2020;27:1604–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Emerson E. Use of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire to assess the mental health needs of children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities. J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2005;30:14–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Douma JCH, Dekker MC, Verhulst FC, Koot HM. Self-Reports on Mental Health Problems of Youth With Moderate to Borderline Intellectual Disabilities. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45:1224–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Taber SM. The veridicality of children’s reports of parenting: A review of factors contributing to parentchild discrepancies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30:999–1010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Achenbach TM, Ruffle TM. The Child Behavior Checklist and Related Forms for Assessing Behavioral/Emotional Problems and Competencies. Pediatr Rev. 2000;21:265–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families, Burlington, VT. 2001.

  45. Fletcher RJ, Barnhill J, Cooper S-A, editors. Diagnostic Manual - Intellectual Disability: A Textbook of Diagnosis of Mental Disorders in Persons with Intellectual Disability. 2nd ed. New York: NADD Press; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Kramer JM, Schwartz A. Reducing Barriers to Patient-Reported Outcome Measures for People With Cognitive Impairments. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2017;98:1705–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Henry LA. The episodic buffer in children with intellectual disabilities: An exploratory study. Res Dev Disabil. 2010;31:1609–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Haynes A, Gilmore L, Shochet I, Campbell M, Roberts C. Factor analysis of the self-report version of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire in a sample of children with intellectual disability. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:847–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Boström P, Johnels JÅ, Thorson M, Broberg M. Subjective Mental Health, Peer Relations, Family, and School Environment in Adolescents with Intellectual Developmental Disorder: A First Report of a New Questionnaire Administered on Tablet PCs. Journal of Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities. 2016;9:207–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Lichtenstein P, Tideman M, Sullivan PF, Serlachius E, Larsson H, Kuja-Halkola R, Butwicka A. Familial risk and heritability of intellectual disability: A population-based cohort study in Sweden. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13560.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Int J Surg. 2021;88: 105906.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Gotzsche PC, et al. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2011;343:d5928–d5928.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: Austria; 2022.

    Google Scholar 

  54. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated development environment for r. RStudio, PBC., Boston, MA. 2020.

  55. Aust F, Barth M. Papaja: Prepare reproducible APA journal articles with R Markdown. 2022.

  56. McGuinness LA, Higgins JP. Risk-of-bias VISualization (robvis): An R package and Shiny web app for visualizing risk-of-bias assessments. Research Synthesis Methods. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Alsem MW, Ketelaar M, Verhoef M. The course of health-related quality of life of preschool children with cerebral palsy. Disabil Rehabil. 2013;35:686–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Anderson DK, Maye MP, Lord C. Changes in Maladaptive Behaviors From Midchildhood to Young Adulthood in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2011;116:381–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Auerbach JG, Gross-Tsur V, Manor O, Shalev RS. Emotional and Behavioral Characteristics Over a Six-Year Period in Youths With Persistent and Nonpersistent Dyscalculia. J Learn Disabil. 2008;41:263–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Baribeau DA, Vigod S, Pullenayegum E, et al. Co-occurring trajectories of anxiety and insistence on sameness behaviour in autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2021;218:20–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Biederman J, Faraone S, Milberger S, et al. A prospective 4-Year follow-up study of attention-deficit hyperactivity and related disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53:437–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Ciciolla L, Gerstein ED, Crnic KA. Reciprocity Among Maternal Distress, Child Behavior, and Parenting: Transactional Processes and Early Childhood Risk. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2014;43:751–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Colvert E, Simonoff E, Capp SJ, Ronald A, Bolton P, Happé F,. Autism Spectrum Disorder and Mental Health Problems: Patterns of Difficulties and Longitudinal Trajectories in a Population-Based Twin Sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05006-8.

  64. Cornish K, Cole V, Longhi E, Karmiloff-Smith A, Scerif G. Does Attention Constrain Developmental Trajectories in Fragile X Syndrome? A 3-Year Prospective Longitudinal Study. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2012;117:103–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Fielding-Gebhardt H, Warren SF, Brady NC. Child Challenging Behavior Influences Maternal Mental Health and Relationship Quality Over Time in Fragile X Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;50:779–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Flouri E, Midouhas E, Charman T, Sarmadi Z. Poverty and the Growth of Emotional and Conduct Problems in Children with Autism With and Without Comorbid ADHD. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:2928–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Gotham K, Brunwasser SM, Lord C. Depressive and Anxiety Symptom Trajectories From School Age Through Young Adulthood in Samples With Autism Spectrum Disorder and Developmental Delay. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54:369-376.e3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Green V, Oreilly M, Itchon J, Sigafoos J. Persistence of early emerging aberrant behavior in children with developmental disabilities. Res Dev Disabil. 2005;26:47–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Harvey EA, Lugo-Candelas CI, Breaux RP. Longitudinal Changes in Individual Symptoms Across the Preschool Years in Children with ADHD. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2015;44:580–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Hauser-Cram P, Woodman AC. Trajectories of Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children with Developmental Disabilities. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2016;44:811–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Hickey EJ, Bolt D, Rodriguez G, Hartley SL. Bidirectional Relations between Parent Warmth and Criticism and the Symptoms and Behavior Problems of Children with Autism. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020;48:865–79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Hogan A, Phillips RL, Howard D, Yiengprugsawan V. Psychosocial outcomes of children with ear infections and hearing problems: A longitudinal study. BMC Pediatr. 2014;14:65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Holmbeck GN, DeLucia C, Essner B, Kelly L, Zebracki K, Friedman D, Jandasek B. Trajectories of psychosocial adjustment in adolescents with spina bifida: A 6-Year, four-wave longitudinal follow-up. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2010;78:511–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Horbach J, Mayer A, Scharke W, Heim S, Günther T. Development of Behavior Problems in Children with and without Specific Learning Disorders in Reading and Spelling from Kindergarten to Fifth Grade. Sci Stud Read. 2020;24:57–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Hoza B, Murray-Close D, Arnold LE, Hinshaw SP, Hechtman L, The MTA Cooperative Group. Time-dependent changes in positively biased self-perceptions of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A developmental psychopathology perspective. Dev Psychopathol. 2010;22:375–90.

  76. Hunsche MC, Saqui S, Mirenda P, et al. Parent-Reported Rates and Clinical Correlates of Suicidality in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Longitudinal Study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;50:3496–509.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Kates WR, Mariano MA, Antshel KM, et al. Trajectories of psychiatric diagnoses and medication usage in youth with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: A 9-Year longitudinal study. Psychol Med. 2019;49:1914–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Lahey BB, Lee SS, Sibley MH, Applegate B, Molina BSG, Pelham WE. Predictors of adolescent outcomes among 4-year-old children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125:168–81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Li B, Bos MG, Stockmann L, Rieffe C. Emotional functioning and the development of internalizing and externalizing problems in young boys with and without autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2020;24:200–10.

  80. Geoff Lindsay, Dockrell JE, Strand Steve. Longitudinal patterns of behaviour problems in children with specific speech and language difficulties: Child and contextual factors. Br J Educ Psychol. 2007;77:811–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Midouhas E, Yogaratnam A, Flouri E, Charman T. Psychopathology Trajectories of Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder: The Role of Family Poverty and Parenting. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52:1057-1065.e1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Moskowitz LJ, Will EA, Black CJ, Roberts JE. Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors in Males and Females with Fragile X Syndrome: Developmental Trajectories in Toddlers Through Young Adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;50:3957–66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Mrug S, Molina BSG, Hoza B, Gerdes AC, Hinshaw SP, Hechtman L, Arnold LE. Peer Rejection and Friendships in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Contributions to Long-Term Outcomes. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2012;40:1013–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Murray-Close D, Hoza B, Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE, Swanson J, Jensen PS, Hechtman L, Wells K. Developmental processes in peer problems of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in The Multimodal Treatment Study of Children With ADHD: Developmental cascades and vicious cycles. Dev Psychopathol. 2010;22:785–802.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Musser ED, Karalunas SL, Dieckmann N, Peris TS, Nigg JT. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder developmental trajectories related to parental expressed emotion. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125:182–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Peverill S, Smith IM, Duku E, et al. Developmental Trajectories of Feeding Problems in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Pediatr Psychol. 2019;44:988–98.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Rai D, Culpin I, Heuvelman H, Magnusson CMK, Carpenter P, Jones HJ, Emond AM, Zammit S, Golding J, Pearson RM. Association of Autistic Traits With Depression From Childhood to Age 18 Years. JAMA Psychiat. 2018;75:835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Rosema S, Muscara F, Anderson V, Godfrey C, Hearps S, Catroppa C. The Trajectory of Long-Term Psychosocial Development 16 Years following Childhood Traumatic Brain Injury. J Neurotrauma. 2015;32:976–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Sigafoos J. Communication Development and Aberrant Behavior in Children with Developmental Disabilities. Educ Train Ment Retard Dev Disabil. 2000;35:168–76.

    Google Scholar 

  90. Sipal RF, Schuengel C, Voorman JM, Van Eck M, Becher JG. Course of behaviour problems of children with cerebral palsy: The role of parental stress and support. Child: Care. Health and Development. 2010;36:74–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. St Clair MC, Pickles A, Durkin K, Conti-Ramsden G. A longitudinal study of behavioral, emotional and social difficulties in individuals with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). J Commun Disord. 2011;44:186–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Steinhausen H-C, Drechsler R, Földényi M, Imhof K, Brandeis D. Clinical Course of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder From Childhood Toward Early Adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42:1085–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Stringer D, Kent R, Briskman J, Lukito S, Charman T, Baird G, Lord C, Pickles A, Simonoff E. Trajectories of emotional and behavioral problems from childhood to early adult life. Autism : the international journal of research and practice. 2020;24:1011–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Tan SS, van Meeteren J, Ketelaar M, Schuengel C, Reinders-Messelink HA, Raat H, Dallmeijer AJ, Roebroeck ME. Long-Term Trajectories of Health-Related Quality of Life in Individuals With Cerebral Palsy: A Multicenter Longitudinal Study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2014;95:2029–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Vaillancourt T, Haltigan JD, Smith I, et al. Joint trajectories of internalizing and externalizing problems in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2017;29:203–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Van keer I, Vandesande S, Dhondt A, Maes B. Changes in the social-emotional functioning of young children with a significant cognitive and motor developmental delay across a two-year period. Int J Dev Disabil. 2021;68:1–13.

  97. Vaughn S, Zaragoza N, Hogan A, Walker J. A Four-Year Longitudinal Investigation of the Social Skills and Behavior Problems of Students with Learning Disabilities. J Learn Disabil. 1993;26:404–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Vaughn S, Haager D. Social Competence as a Multifaceted Construct: How do Students with Learning Disabilities Fare? Learn Disabil Q. 1994;17:253–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Wall CA, Hogan AL, Will EA, McQuillin S, Kelleher BL, Roberts JE. Early negative affect in males and females with fragile X syndrome: Implications for anxiety and autism. J Neurodev Disord. 2019;11:22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. Wei X, Yu JW, Shaver D. Longitudinal Effects of ADHD in Children with Learning Disabilities or Emotional Disturbances. Except Child. 2014;80:205–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Williams KE, Sciberras E. Sleep and Self-Regulation from Birth to 7 Years: A Retrospective Study of Children with and Without Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder at 8 to 9 Years. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2016;37:385–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Woodman AC, Mawdsley HP, Hauser-Cram P. Parenting stress and child behavior problems within families of children with developmental disabilities: Transactional relations across 15 years. Res Dev Disabil. 2015;36:264–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Woodruff-Borden J, Kistler DJ, Henderson DR, Crawford NA, Mervis CB. Longitudinal course of anxiety in children and adolescents with Williams syndrome. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet. 2010;154C:277–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  104. Yeates KO, Taylor HG. Behavior Problems in School and Their Educational Correlates Among Children With Traumatic Brain Injury. Exceptionality. 2006;14:141–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Zendarski N, Mensah F, Hiscock H, Sciberras E. Trajectories of Emotional and Conduct Problems and Their Association With Early High School Achievement and Engagement for Adolescents With ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2021;25:623–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Sasser TR, Kalvin CB, Bierman KL. Developmental trajectories of clinically significant attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms from grade 3 through 12 in a high-risk sample: Predictors and outcomes. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125:207–19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  107. Lee J. Maternal stress, well-being, and impaired sleep in mothers of children with developmental disabilities: A literature review. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:4255–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Stewart ME, Barnard L, Pearson J, Hasan R, O’Brien G. Presentation of depression in autism and Asperger syndrome: A review. Autism. 2006;10:103–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Kaufmann WE, Kidd SA, Andrews HF, et al. Autism Spectrum Disorder in Fragile X Syndrome: Cooccurring Conditions and Current Treatment. Pediatrics. 2017;139:S194–206.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Mats Granlund for his comments on the manuscript.

Funding

Open access funding provided by Linköping University. The current study was conducted within the CHILD-PMH project, which was made possible through funding from the Swedish Research Council (2018-05824_VR).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors made the following contributions. Magnus Ivarsson: Conceptualisation, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Visualisation, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing; Henrik Danielsson: Conceptualisation, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing—review & editing; Christine Imms: Conceptualisation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing—review & editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Magnus Ivarsson.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ivarsson, M., Danielsson, H. & Imms, C. Measurement issues in longitudinal studies of mental health problems in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. BMC Psychol 13, 267 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-025-02450-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-025-02450-4

Keywords